首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   17142篇
  免费   824篇
  国内免费   15篇
  17981篇
  2021年   138篇
  2020年   236篇
  2019年   250篇
  2018年   374篇
  2017年   374篇
  2016年   379篇
  2015年   299篇
  2014年   321篇
  2013年   1357篇
  2012年   581篇
  2011年   657篇
  2010年   380篇
  2009年   359篇
  2008年   490篇
  2007年   458篇
  2006年   404篇
  2005年   340篇
  2004年   345篇
  2003年   322篇
  2002年   330篇
  2001年   703篇
  2000年   700篇
  1999年   462篇
  1998年   180篇
  1997年   174篇
  1996年   139篇
  1995年   135篇
  1993年   138篇
  1992年   360篇
  1991年   321篇
  1990年   345篇
  1989年   334篇
  1988年   318篇
  1987年   288篇
  1986年   311篇
  1985年   298篇
  1984年   236篇
  1983年   200篇
  1979年   192篇
  1978年   161篇
  1975年   189篇
  1974年   244篇
  1973年   199篇
  1972年   204篇
  1971年   168篇
  1970年   149篇
  1969年   199篇
  1968年   203篇
  1967年   163篇
  1966年   182篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
271.
Studies of the variables that determine whether an adolescent is placed in the mental health or juvenile justice system for treatment have led to conflicting conclusions based on impressionistic data. The primary hypothesis of this study--that demographic variables would and personality/psychopathology variables would not differentiate into which system a youth will be placed--was supported. Adolescents were studied at intake into both the juvenile justice system and the mental health system. Data were collected on demographics (structured interviews), personality/psychopathology (MMPI), social adjustment (CAAP), and academic achievement (PIAT). A discriminant function analysis identified eight statistically significant variables which differentiated the two groups. In order of decreasing importance they are: ethnicity, gender, MMPI-depression, previous mental health history, CAAP-productivity, drug use, parental marital history, and parental religious preference.  相似文献   
272.
G Margolis  K Lindholm 《Adolescence》1986,21(81):225-234
The authors, a college psychotherapist and dean of students, describe the relationship between these college personnel. They describe the dynamics of their interaction and the importance of communication and mutual understanding. They define counseling and therapy, discuss the issues of agency and confidentiality, suggest methods of referral and consultation, and emphasize the need for support and trust within a school community. Finally, they present case studies that illustrate the benefits of mutual trust, understanding, and respect.  相似文献   
273.
274.
275.
276.
The purpose of the present paper was to determine whether people encode relations among component features in experience, or whether they simply encode the occurrence of features separately as independent cues. This has become a major issue in theories of concept-formation at the present time. It is suggested in this paper that two problems have particularly obstructed a clear resolution of the issue. The first is a consistent failure to check to what extent training materials actually contain relations among features, before results are declared in favour of a particular model. The second is the use, hitherto, of only informal methods of analysing such relations, where they exist, and of predicting from them the structure of the concept. This paper describes an expression for measuring the overall relations among feature-variables in concept training materials. It also introduces a methodology, using log-linear models, for predicting concept structures on the basis of those relations. An experiment combined these methods to assess whether subjects abstracted relations among features, or whether they abstracted independent cues. A subsidiary aim was to assess how feedback, based on independent-cue information, during learning, affected the construction of the concept. The results came out strongly in favour of relational-coding and there was some evidence that feedback based on independent-cues retarded concept formation.  相似文献   
277.
Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
  相似文献   
278.
Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
Four experiments are reported which examined the size of the perceptual span in second-, fourth-, and sixth-grade children, as well as adult skilled readers. The results indicated that the perceptual span in beginning readers is slightly smaller than the perceptual span of skilled readers. Using a moving window technique, it was found that the perceptual span of beginning readers extends about 11 character spaces to the right of fixation; for skilled readers, the span extends 14-15 spaces to the right of fixation. Beginning readers apparently devote more of their processing to the foveally fixated word than more proficient readers, but their perceptual span appears to be asymmetric to the right of fixation as is the case for skilled readers. The results of the experiments also indicated that the size of the perceptual span is variable and can be influenced by the difficulty of the text. It is concluded that the size of the perceptual span does not cause beginning readers' slow reading rates.  相似文献   
279.
Altruism and aggression: the heritability of individual differences   总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8  
Five questionnaires measuring altruistic and aggressive tendencies were completed by 573 adult twin pairs of both sexes from the University of London Institute of Psychiatry Volunteer Twin Register. The questionnaires measured altruism, empathy, nurturance, aggressiveness, and assertiveness. The intraclass correlations for the five scales, respectively, were .53, .54, .49, .40, and .52 for 296 monozygotic pairs, and .25, .20, .14, .04, and .20 for 179 same-sex dizygotic pairs, resulting in broad heritability estimates of 56%, 68%, 70%, 72%, and 64%. Additional analyses, using maximum-likelihood model-fitting, revealed approximately 50% of the variance on each scale to be associated with genetic effects, virtually 0% with the twins' common environment, and the remaining 50% with each twins' specific environment and/or error associated with the test. Correcting for the unreliability in the tests raised the maximum-likelihood heritabilities to approximately 60%. Age and sex differences were also found: altruism increased over the age span from 19 to 60, whereas aggressiveness decreased, and, at each age, women had higher scores than men on altruism and lower scores on aggressiveness.  相似文献   
280.
Theories of observing differ in predicting whether or not a signal for absence of reinforcement (S−) is capable of reinforcing observing responses. Experiments in which S− was first removed from and then restored to the procedure have yielded mixed results. The present experiments suggest that failure to control for the direct effect of presenting S− may have been responsible. Pigeons and operant procedures were used. Experiment 1 showed that presentations of S−, even when not contingent on observing, can raise the rate of an observing response that was reinforced only by presentations of a signal (S+) that accompanied a schedule of food delivery. Experiment 2 showed that this effect resulted from bursts of responding that followed offsets of S−. Experiment 3 showed that, when the presence of S− was held constant, lower rates occurred when S− was dependent on, rather than independent of, observing. These results support theories that characterize S− as incapable of reinforcing observing responses.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号