全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1919篇 |
免费 | 54篇 |
专业分类
1973篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 34篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 50篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 200篇 |
2012年 | 60篇 |
2011年 | 80篇 |
2010年 | 59篇 |
2009年 | 50篇 |
2008年 | 72篇 |
2007年 | 69篇 |
2006年 | 70篇 |
2005年 | 61篇 |
2004年 | 71篇 |
2003年 | 70篇 |
2002年 | 75篇 |
2001年 | 31篇 |
2000年 | 33篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 31篇 |
1997年 | 27篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 44篇 |
1994年 | 30篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 34篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 23篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1973条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
A recall advantage for common words was not evidenced when subjects generated or rated the list words on the basis of commonness and on a semantic, phonological, or orthographic basis prior to recall. Recall of the same set of words following only structural judgments of the words resulted in the usual recall advantage for common words. Results were not consistent with interpretations based only on the associative strengths of the recalled items. A process based on categorization of the words to be recalled was suggested as an alternative explanation. 相似文献
52.
Given the assumptions that the taste sense is analytic and that the total perceived intensity of a complex taste equals the sum of the perceived intensities of the components, a new procedure was devised to scale taste intensity. Subjects matched the intensity of test substances either to the total intensity or to the intensity of one component of two-component mixtures tin which the two components appeared equally intense. Given the above-stated assumptions, such a pair of matches should define a psychological ratio of 2∶1. Scales erected by this procedure agreed well with scales determined by magnitude estimation. 相似文献
53.
Linda I. Garrity 《Memory & cognition》1977,5(6):615-622
Electromyographic studies of subvocal speech are evaluated with two different aims: (1)a methodological review of controls for nonspeech artifact and of data analysis techniques, and (2) a review of substantive findings and suggestions for future research. A highly effective procedure, involving manipulation of phoneme content in certain trials, has been developed to control nonspeech artifact generated by the speech musculature. The commonly used data analysis technique of amplitude measurement of the single highest polygraph pen deflection is inadequate. Techniques which take frequency into account, such as analog computer integration of voltage values, are more sensitive. Even greater sensitivity is needed to isolate individual words so that rehearsal strategies can be investigated. It has been clearly established that preschool children as young as age 4 engage in spontaneous subvocalization that is related to recall in boys but not in girls. 相似文献
54.
55.
A reinforcement system utilizing instructions, modelling, feedback, and group reinforcement was employed in an attempt to reduce disruptive noise on three university residence halls. A fourth hall received the same treatment program without the reinforcement component. Noise scores were determined by recording the number of discrete noise occurrences over a criterion decibel level. On all four residential floors, noise scores during treatment conditions were lower than initial and final baseline levels. Additionally, periods of noise reduction corresponded to the changing criterion multiple-baseline and reversal designs utilized. Pre- and posttreatment questionnaire responses from the three reinforcement floors paralleled changes in objective noise data. At posttreatment, residents reported less noise disturbance of study and sleep and more control over the noise situation and floor problems in general. These results indicated that a comprehensive behavior-modification treatment package was effective in reducing disruptive noise in university residence halls. Difficulties in data collection and anomalies in the data are discussed. Future directions for field-based behavior-modification research are outlined. 相似文献
56.
Dr. Valerie Klinge Thomas Rodziewicz Linda Schwartz 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1976,4(1):73-81
The present study compared the WISC and WISC-R on a group of psychiatric adolescent inpatients. The results indicated high intercorrelations between the two test versions but with the correlations linearly related to the changes in content and administration rules of the WISC-R. Analyses of variance indicated that, on the basis of the Full Scale IQ, the WISC is an easier test than the WISC-R.The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Ms. Kathleen Lennox for her aid in data analysis and computer programming. 相似文献
57.
58.
Siobhan E Colgan Elizabeth Lanter Cara McComish Linda R Watson Elizabeth R Crais Grace T Baranek 《Child neuropsychology》2006,12(4-5):307-319
This study analyzes the emergent use of gestures used among 9-12-month-old infants with autism and typical development using retrospective video analysis. The purpose of the present investigation was to examine the frequency, initiation, prompting, and diversity of types of gestures used for social interaction purposes. It was hypothesized that a restricted variety in type(s) of gestures as well as fewer child-initiated gestures and more prompted gestures would be associated with later diagnosis of autism. Logistic regression analysis found that decreased variety in type of gestures used was significantly associated with autism status. Neither number of total gestures nor initiation of gestures (child-initiated vs. prompted) was significantly associated with autism status. 相似文献
59.
Gregory PT Gregory LA 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2006,120(3):262-268
A common response of animals to physical restraint is tonic immobility. The authors observed the behavior of garter snakes, Thamnophis elegans, in the field to determine the frequency of immobility as a response to handling. Most snakes fled after release, but the remainder remained immobile, sometimes on their backs (supination), for up to 10 min. Immobility was seen most often in pregnant snakes, which also were more likely than other snakes not to have moved before capture. Failure to move, either before or after capture, might be a consequence of the limited locomotory ability of gravid snakes. However, the lack of observations of interactions between snakes and their natural predators impedes researchers' understanding of the antipredator value, if any, of tonic immobility. 相似文献
60.