全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1992篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
1999篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 50篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 25篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 200篇 |
2012年 | 60篇 |
2011年 | 80篇 |
2010年 | 60篇 |
2009年 | 51篇 |
2008年 | 72篇 |
2007年 | 69篇 |
2006年 | 70篇 |
2005年 | 61篇 |
2004年 | 71篇 |
2003年 | 70篇 |
2002年 | 76篇 |
2001年 | 31篇 |
2000年 | 34篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 33篇 |
1997年 | 28篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 45篇 |
1994年 | 31篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 34篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 23篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1999条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
This study explores the relationship between wife's employment, sex-role traditionalism, and the reported division of household tasks and decisions. Data were collected from three samples of currently married women between the ages of 18 and 49 in Los Angeles County. Sex-role traditionalism was positively related to wives' relative performance of feminine tasks, but generally unrelated to masculine task performance and decision-making between spouses. Employed or professional women took primary responsibility for relatively fewer feminine tasks than did nonemployed or nonprofessional women. The findings suggest that despite differences among women in sex-role traditionalism and employment status, most women say that they and their spouses divide tasks in a traditional sex-role fashion. 相似文献
162.
Adolescent Substance Use: Preliminary Examinations of School and Neighborhood Context 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Kevin W. Allison Isiaah Crawford Peter E. Leone Edison Trickett Alina Perez-Febles Linda M. Burton Ree Le Blanc 《American journal of community psychology》1999,27(2):111-141
In considering the influences of microsystems on adolescent substance use, familial and peer contexts have received the most extensive attention in the research literature. School and neighborhood settings, however, are other developmental contexts that may exert specific influences on adolescent substance use. In many instances, school settings are organized to provide educational services to students who share similar educational abilities and behavioral repertoires. The resulting segregation of students into these settings may result in different school norms for substance use. Similarly, neighborhood resources, including models for substance use and drug sales involvement, may play an important role in adolescent substance use. We briefly review literature examining contextual influences on adolescent substance use, and present results from two preliminary studies examining the contribution of school and neighborhood context to adolescent substance use. In the first investigation, we examine the impact of familial, peer, and school contexts on adolescent substance use. Respondents were 283 students (ages 13 to 18) from regular and special education classrooms in six schools. Although peer and parental contexts were important predictors of substance use, school norms for drug use accounted for variance in adolescent use beyond that explained by peer and parental norms. Data from a second study of 114 adolescents (mean age = 15) examines neighborhood contributions to adolescent substance use. In this sample, neighborhood indices did not contribute to our understanding of adolescent substance use. Implications for prevention are presented. 相似文献
163.
164.
Gender and Sex-Role Influences on Children''s Self-Esteem 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This study examined direct and moderating influences of gender and sex-role orientations on children's general self-esteem. Moderating influences of these variables on the prediction of self-esteem were examined with respect to two sets of competence beliefs regarding school achievement: perceived capacities and perceived strategies for doing well in school. One hundred nineteen fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade children were assessed using the Perceived Competence Scale for Children (Harter, 1982), the Multidimensional Measure of Children's Perceptions of Control (Connell, 1985), and the Children's Personal Attributes Questionnaire (Hall & Halberstadt, 1980). Correlational and hierarchical multiple regression analyses indicated that upper elementary schoolchildren's general self-esteem is (a) marginally related to biological gender, with boys showing a slight advantage; (b) significantly related to masculinity and androgyny; and (c) predicted more strongly by perceived capacities to do schoolwork in girls than in boys, and by perceived (lack of) strategies for academic success in nontraditionally sex-typed children than in traditionally sex-typed children. Of the two nontraditionally sex-typed groups, androgynous children were found to have more positive school competence beliefs than were undifferentiated children. 相似文献
165.
Linda Baker 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1984,38(2):289-311
Nine- and eleven-year-old children differing in reading proficiency read and commented on brief expository passages containing three different types of embedded problems (nonsense words, prior knowledge violations, and internal inconsistencies). Half of the children were specifically instructed as to the types of standards they should apply in order to detect the problems (lexical, external consistency, and internal consistency); the remaining children were simply instructed to look for problems. Both quantitative and qualitative differences in standard use were revealed by the children's comments about all parts of the passages. Older and better readers used more different standards and they used them more frequently than younger and poorer readers. The lexical standard was more likely to be adopted spontaneously than the other two standards and it was the only standard used by a substantial proportion of both younger and poorer readers. Finally, the consistent effects of instruction type indicate that children's evaluation activities were strongly influenced by the amount of guidance received. 相似文献
166.
167.
Sue Ann K. Bauserman Ileana Arias W. Edward Craighead 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1995,17(3):231-249
Causal attributions (i.e., locus, stability, globality) and responsibility attributions (i.e., bad intent, selfish motivation, blame) were assessed in the spouses of 27 depressed psychiatric inpatients and 30 nondepressed dyads to test predictions derived from Hooley's (1987) symptom-controllability model of marital distress. Results indicated that (1) depressed patients and their spouses were less dyadically adjusted than nondepressed spouses, (2) causal and responsibility attributions about depressive behaviors predicted lower dyadic adjustment, and (3) attributions of causality mediated the relationship between group status (depressed or nondepressed) and dyadic adjustment among spouses who had higher expectations for their partner to change. Results suggest that among spouses with a high expectancy for change, depression may be a risk factor for marital distress. 相似文献
168.
John D. Teasdale Barbara H. Dritschel Melanie J. Taylor Linda Proctor Charlotte A. Lloyd Ian Nimmo-Smith Alan D. Baddeley 《Memory & cognition》1995,23(5):551-559
Stimulus-independent thoughts (SITs) are streams of thoughts and images unrelated to immediate sensory input. Four experiments examined the contribution of aspects of working memory to production of SITs. In Experiments 1 and 2, interventions that were targeted on, respectively, phonological and visuospatial components of working memory both interfered with production of SITs, but there was evidence that these tasks also made demands on central executive resources. Experiments 3 and 4 specifically examined the hypothesis that production of SITs and control of nonproceduralized tasks both depend on central executive resources, and so should show mutual interference. In Experiment 3, prior practice on pursuit rotor and memory tasks reduced the interference with SITs from concurrent task performance. In Experiment 4, randomness within a task involving random-number generation was less when SITs were being produced concurrently than it was when they were not. The results suggest that production of SITs depends on central executive resources. 相似文献
169.
Research suggests that males outperform females on standardized tests of math ability from early adolescence onward. Several explanations for this difference focus on the different experiences of females and males with numerical information. We reasoned that males have more experience with numerical information than females because typical male interests (e.g., sports) are more likely to involve numerical information than typical female interests (e.g., physical appearance). Thus, we predicted that males would attend more to numerical information than would females, at least when the information was presented in a male-related or gender-neutral context. Females were expected to attend more than males when the context was female-related. Results using White/Caucasian subjects provided some support for these predictions. Implications for future research on the relationship between the gender-linkage of math materials and gender differences in math performance are discussed. 相似文献
170.