全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2877篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
2904篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 41篇 |
2019年 | 49篇 |
2018年 | 66篇 |
2017年 | 63篇 |
2016年 | 59篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 61篇 |
2013年 | 288篇 |
2012年 | 91篇 |
2011年 | 144篇 |
2010年 | 85篇 |
2009年 | 86篇 |
2008年 | 119篇 |
2007年 | 115篇 |
2006年 | 96篇 |
2005年 | 88篇 |
2004年 | 99篇 |
2003年 | 94篇 |
2002年 | 98篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 58篇 |
1999年 | 45篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 40篇 |
1995年 | 58篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 40篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 32篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 31篇 |
1985年 | 50篇 |
1984年 | 39篇 |
1983年 | 50篇 |
1982年 | 39篇 |
1981年 | 44篇 |
1980年 | 27篇 |
1979年 | 31篇 |
1978年 | 29篇 |
1977年 | 28篇 |
1976年 | 25篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2904条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
Helena M. Blumen Suparna Rajaram Linda Henkel 《Journal of applied research in memory and cognition》2013,2(2):107-117
Our aim in this paper is to bring attention to the applied value of collaborative memory research in aging. At this time, much collaborative memory research focuses on the negative effects of collaboration in younger adults, and is primarily basic in nature. Here, we highlight the positive effects of collaboration that have received less attention, with a particular emphasis on the applied value of these effects in older adults. We first review studies to show that recalling in a group improves later individual recall and reduces memory errors in older adults. We then outline a four-step approach towards bridging laboratory and applied collaborative memory research, which involves: (1) complementing traditional paradigms with more ecologically valid paradigms, (2) evaluating these paradigms in applied settings, (3) adapting these paradigms for use with cognitively intact and cognitively impaired populations, and (4) modifying these paradigms to examine the neural systems that operate during collaborative recall. 相似文献
152.
LeBlanc LA Coates AM Daneshvar S Charlop-Christy MH Morris C Lancaster BM 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2003,36(2):253-257
We evaluated video modeling and reinforcement for teaching perspective-taking skills to 3 children with autism using a multiple baseline design. Video modeling and reinforcement were effective; however, only 2 children were able to pass an untrained task, indicating limited generalization. The findings suggest that video modeling may be an effective technology for teaching perspective taking if researchers can continue to develop strategies for enhancing the generalization of these new skills. 相似文献
153.
154.
155.
Schaie KW Caskie GI Revell AJ Willis SL Kaszniak AW Teri L 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2005,12(3):245-277
A battery of 17 neuropsychological tests (including the CERAD battery) and 17 psychometric ability tests were administered to a sample of 499 participants of the Seattle Longitudinal study who had been given the psychometric ability tests seven and 14 years earlier. The neuropsychological tests were projected into a 5-factor psychometric ability space by means of extension analysis. The concurrent regressions of the neuropsychology tests on the psychometric ability tests were then used to estimate neuropsychology test scores from the psychometric ability tests administered in 1984, 1991 and 1998. Neuropsychologists then rated the study participants as either normal, suspect or cognitively impaired in 1998. Changes in estimated test scores were computed over seven and fourteen years. Significant odds ratios between normal and cognitively impaired groups were found for all neuropsychological tests over the proximal period and for most tests over the 14-year period. Similar findings occurred for the odds ratios between the normal and suspect groups for the most proximal 7-year changes. 相似文献
156.
Research on affective dimensions of intergroup relationships suggests that positive effects of intergroup contact can generalize through establishing affective ties with outgroup members. However, research on cognitive dimensions emphasizes that it is often difficult to generalize positive contact outcomes. In this research, the authors examine whether affective and cognitive dimensions of prejudice bear different relationships to intergroup contact. Using data from a larger meta-analysis of contact effects, Study 1 demonstrates that affective indicators of prejudice typically yield stronger, inverse contact-prejudice relationships than such cognitive indicators as stereotypes. Study 2 replicates these trends in a survey study using multiple indicators of affective and cognitive dimensions of prejudice. Study 2 also shows significant, inverse relationships between contact and affective prejudice when contact is assessed either as number of outgroup friends or intergroup closeness. Together, these results suggest that affective dimensions of intergroup relationships are especially critical for understanding the nature of contact-prejudice effects. 相似文献
157.
Gallo LC Bogart LM Vranceanu AM Matthews KA 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2005,88(2):386-399
The current study used ecological momentary assessment to test several tenets of the reserve capacity model (L.C. Gallo & K. A. Matthews, 2003). Women (N = 108) with varying socioeconomic status (SES) monitored positive and negative psychosocial experiences and emotions across 2 days. Measures of intrapsychic and social resources were aggregated to represent the reserve capacity available to manage stress. Lower SES was associated with less perceived control and positive affect and more social strain. Control and strain contributed to the association between SES and positive affect. Lower SES elicited greater positive but not negative emotional reactivity to psychosocial experiences. Women with low SES had fewer resources relative to those with higher SES, and resources contributed to the association between SES and daily experiences. 相似文献
158.
The Toulmin diagram layout is very familiar and widely used, particularly in the teaching of critical thinking skills. The
conventional box-and-arrow diagram is equally familiar and widespread. Translation between the two throws up a number of interesting
challenges. Some of these challenges (such as the relationship between Toulmin warrants and their counterparts in traditional
diagrams) represent slightly different ways of looking at old and deep theoretical questions. Others (such as how to allow
Toulmin diagrams to be recursive) are diagrammatic versions of questions that have already been addressed in artificial intelligence
models of argument. But there are further questions (such as the relationships between refutations, rebuttals and undercutters,
and the roles of multiple warrants) that are posed as a specific result of examining the diagram inter-translation problem.
These three classes of problems are discussed. To the first class are addressed solutions based on engineering pragmatism;
to the second class, are addressed solutions drawn from the appropriate literature; and to the third class, fuller exploration
is offered justifying the approaches taken in developing solutions that offer both pragmatic utility and theoretical interest.
Finally, these solutions are explored briefly in the context of the Araucaria system, showing the ways in which analysts can tackle arguments either using one diagrammatic style or another, or even a
combination of the two. 相似文献
159.
William A. Roberts Nicole A. Guitar Heidi L. Marsh Hayden MacDonald 《Animal cognition》2016,19(3):593-604
The interaction of working and reference memory was studied in rats on an eight-arm radial maze. In two experiments, rats were trained to perform working memory and reference memory tasks. On working memory trials, they were allowed to enter four randomly chosen arms for reward in a study phase and then had to choose the unentered arms for reward in a test phase. On reference memory trials, they had to learn to visit the same four arms on the maze on every trial for reward. Retention was tested on working memory trials in which the interval between the study and test phase was 15 s, 15 min, or 30 min. At each retention interval, tests were performed in which the correct WM arms were either congruent or incongruent with the correct RM arms. Both experiments showed that congruency interacted with retention interval, yielding more forgetting at 30 min on incongruent trials than on congruent trials. The effect of reference memory strength on the congruency effect was examined in Experiment 1, and the effect of associating different contexts with working and reference memory on the congruency effect was studied in Experiment 2. 相似文献
160.
Lorne M. Sulsky Joel Marcus Heather A. MacDonald 《Journal of business and psychology》2016,31(3):383-398