首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3131篇
  免费   103篇
  国内免费   2篇
  3236篇
  2022年   19篇
  2021年   25篇
  2020年   40篇
  2019年   52篇
  2018年   73篇
  2017年   80篇
  2016年   60篇
  2015年   50篇
  2014年   56篇
  2013年   311篇
  2012年   107篇
  2011年   129篇
  2010年   87篇
  2009年   89篇
  2008年   111篇
  2007年   122篇
  2006年   117篇
  2005年   106篇
  2004年   115篇
  2003年   111篇
  2002年   123篇
  2001年   48篇
  2000年   55篇
  1999年   56篇
  1998年   46篇
  1997年   46篇
  1996年   49篇
  1995年   70篇
  1994年   54篇
  1993年   51篇
  1992年   46篇
  1991年   41篇
  1990年   37篇
  1989年   38篇
  1988年   33篇
  1987年   29篇
  1986年   35篇
  1985年   45篇
  1984年   45篇
  1983年   49篇
  1982年   35篇
  1981年   53篇
  1980年   34篇
  1979年   41篇
  1978年   40篇
  1977年   38篇
  1976年   23篇
  1975年   29篇
  1974年   17篇
  1973年   16篇
排序方式: 共有3236条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
901.
In choice reaction time (RT) tasks, college students verified the truth of displays expressing spatial relations between two objects. The relations werelocational (A is left of B) ororientational (A and B are horizontal). The objects were names of states in the United States, symbols, or letter arrays. The objects were memorized prior to the display (states and letters) or were presented as part of the display (symbols and letters). In the location tasks with both states and symbols, locatives were spatial (right, left, above, below) or compass (north, south, east, west). Distance between states was also varied. When location was judged, horizontally aligned stimuli resulted in slower responses than vertically aligned stimuli, independently of materials and locative set. Reaction time was inversely related to distance. When orientation was judged, responses to horizontal pairs of states were slower than responses to vertical pairs of states, responses to horizontal pairs of letters were faster than responses to vertical pairs, and RT did not depend upon the orientation of symbols. This pattern of results suggests that orientational judgments are influenced by type of materials and the entext to which the material has been encoded (i.e., memorized). Locational judgments reflect a potent source of difficulty not present in orientation tasks, namely, telling left from right. Alternative explanations of the right-left effect are discussed.  相似文献   
902.
In two experiments, tape-recorded strings of seven recall digits were preceded by a redundant stimulus prefix containing either one or three digits. The prefix and recall digits (1) were articulatorily continuous, or (2)were made discontinuous by tape-splicing, or (3)were made discontinuous by deliberately spoken prosodic contour. Subjects were instructed to ignore the redundant prefixes and report only the recall digits. The main results were: (1)Compared to strings preceded by no prefix, all prefix conditions reduced recall. (2)Three-digit prefixes reduced recall less than one-digit prefixes. (3) In most cases, spliced prefixes reduced recall less than intact prefixes. Acoustic waveform measurements are presented to provide a partial account for the results.  相似文献   
903.
In a report paradigm, two letters are presented on a trial which are either confusable(e.g., P and R) or nonconfusable (e.g., P and M) in terms of visual features. Across trials, interletter distance, retinal location, duration, and visual field are varied. Identification accuracy on confusable trials was generally lower than on nonconfusable trials, and this effect of level of confusability increased with distance from the fixation point, decreased with duration, and was smaller on the central letter than on the more peripheral letter. A quantitative model, incorporating aspects of the interactive channels model (Estes, 1972) and feature perturbation model (Wolford, 1975), is developed and tested. One parameter of the model measures the effective similarity between two letters after lateral inhibition has occurred, and other parameters measure the probability of feature perturbation in foveal and peripheral directions.  相似文献   
904.
The present experiments were concerned with subsyllabic units in word recognition. The studies employed a same-different RT task. A one-syllable word and a probe were presented simultaneously, and subjects responded “same” if the probe was an integral subset of the word (BLAST:BL), and “different” otherwise (BLAST:BC). Probes included single letters (BLAST:L), doublets (BLAST:LA), triplets (BLAST:LAS), and the whole word (BLAST: BLAST). The first experiment suggested that a variety of spelling patterns, as well as the whole word, served as units of processing. In particular, initial consonant clusters were processed as quickly as single letters, and only slightly faster than the whole words. Consonant strings were used in the second experiment, which demonstrated spelling pattern effects with permissible strings (BLGST:BL) as compared to nonpermissable strings (BXJPK:BX). A third experiment demonstrated that the particular effective units depend on task demands.  相似文献   
905.
906.
Stimulus competition was studied in college students' correlational judgments in a medical decision-making setting. In accord with prior findings, subjects making cause-to-effect (predictive) judgments discounted a stimulus event that was moderately correlated with a target event when rival stimuli were more highly correlated with the effect. However, subjects making effect-to-cause (diagnostic) judgments were not at all disposed to discount a stimulus event which was moderately correlated with a target event when rival stimuli were more highly correlated with the cause. The theoretical implications of these results are considered in connection with associative and mentalistic models of causal attribution.  相似文献   
907.
The role of adults' social cognitions in mediating judgments of hyperactive children's medicationrelated behavior change was explored. Two hundred eightyeight undergraduates observed two videotaped excerpts of a hyperactive target boy playing a group game with two peers. Each target was taking either methylphenidate (0.6 mg/kg) during both excerpts, placebo during both excerpts, methylphenidate first followed by placebo, or placebo first followed by methylphenidate. Adults' cumulative social evaluations of the child were assessed after they viewed both video segments. Results indicated that observers combined their perceptions of the two behavior samples into composite impressions using an equalweight averaging algorithm. Even for children whose behavior improved, adults' ratings of undercontrolled behaviors continued to meet or, in some cases exceed, research cutoff scores used to identify hyperactive children. The findings suggest that the actual behaviors of children with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) play a more influential role in shaping interpersonal impressions than do perceiver socialcognitive processes such as primacy, recency, or integration biases.This project was supported in part by a UC Regents' Dissertation fellowship to Douglas A. Granger while at the University of California at Irvine. We acknowledge Tom Crawford and Karen Rook for their helpful comments at formative stages of this project, and support from the UCLA Fernald Child Study Center and Program in Psychoneuroimmunology. We thank behavior observers Leslie Klein, Jeanne Tsai, and Dawn Rowland, and data managers Pam Ajang and Scott Gutentag. A preliminary report of this study was presented at the 1991 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.  相似文献   
908.
In this study the authors examined relationships between trainees' cognitive style, program membership, amount of practicum experience, and needs and expectations for supervision. Participants were 57 trainees in three graduate programs at a midwestern university. All respondents completed the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; Myers & McCaulley, 1985), the trainee version of the Supervisor Perception Form (SPF-T; Heppner & Roehlke, 1984), and the Supervisory Needs subtest of the Counselor Development Questionnaire (CDQ-SN; Reising & Daniels, 1983). Results suggested that trainees' expectations and needs regarding the supervisory experience differed by amount of practicum experience, cognitive style, and program membership. Differences due to amount of experience were more prevalent than were those due to cognitive style or to program membership.  相似文献   
909.
910.
Explanations for the rise in demand for counselling are discussed. These include dissatisfaction with clinical psychology, the loss of social and personal supports, New Age aspirations for personal development, and insuficient numbers of clinical psychologists. Evidence is presented against the views that there has been a recent loss of social or personal supports, and that personal-development aspirations through unorthodox routes are a new phenomenon. The apparent rise in demand for counselling is explained rather as a rise in demand for counselling training, by would-be practitioners. This demand in turn is fuelled by developments in public-sector employent, leading employees to seek extra qualifications to enhance their job security. Nevertheless, the relatively weak appeal of clinical psychology, together with its low public visibility and its weak licensing power, encourage the continual emergence of alternative therapies.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号