全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2256篇 |
免费 | 43篇 |
专业分类
2299篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 36篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 47篇 |
2016年 | 54篇 |
2015年 | 44篇 |
2014年 | 49篇 |
2013年 | 264篇 |
2012年 | 62篇 |
2011年 | 80篇 |
2010年 | 47篇 |
2009年 | 59篇 |
2008年 | 79篇 |
2007年 | 85篇 |
2006年 | 93篇 |
2005年 | 89篇 |
2004年 | 49篇 |
2003年 | 62篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 46篇 |
2000年 | 43篇 |
1999年 | 59篇 |
1998年 | 47篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 30篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 21篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 27篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 32篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 29篇 |
1981年 | 23篇 |
1979年 | 29篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 21篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
1972年 | 15篇 |
1970年 | 17篇 |
1968年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有2299条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
52.
Peter J. Lewis 《Synthese》2001,129(3):371-380
Putnam and Laudan separately argue that the falsity of past scientific theories gives us reason to doubt the truth of current theories. Their arguments have been highly influential, and have generated a significant literature over the past couple of decades. Most of this literature attempts to defend scientific realism by attacking the historical evidence on which the premises of the relevant argument are based. However, I argue that both Putnam's and Laudan's arguments are fallacious, and hence attacking their premises is unnecessary. The paper concludes with a discussion of the further historical evidence that would be required if the pessimistic induction is to present a serious threat to scientific realism. 相似文献
53.
Craig S. Cashwell Amy Skinner Jennifer C. Lewis J. Scott Young Tammy H. Cashwell 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2001,31(3):141-149
An analog design was used to investigate the mediating effects of client locus of control (i.e., an internal focus or external focus) on perceptions of counselor style of interpreting events (i.e., absolute or tentative interpretations) on measures of perceived session depth and smoothness, and perceived counselor expertness, attractiveness, and trustworthiness. One hundred and thirty-four undergraduate college students, crossed on level of locus of control, were randomly assigned to one of two treatment conditions, and then completed measures of perceived session depth and smoothness, as well as perceived counselor expertness, attractiveness, and trustworthiness. Results indicated a significant interaction effect on perceived counselor trustworthiness. Implications for college counselors are provided. 相似文献
54.
Aggression between partners represents a potential guiding force in family dynamics. However, research examining the influence of partner aggression (physically and psychologically aggressive acts by both partners) on harsh parenting and young child adjustment has been limited by a frequent focus on low-risk samples and by the examination of partner aggression at a single time point. Especially in the context of multiple risk factors and around transitions such as childbirth, partner aggression might be better understood as a dynamic process. In the present study, longitudinal trajectories of partner aggression from birth to age 3 years in a large, high-risk, and ethnically diverse sample (N = 461) were examined. Specific risk factors were tested as predictors of aggression over time, and the longitudinal effects of partner aggression on maternal harsh parenting and child maladjustment were examined. Partner aggression decreased over time, with higher maternal depression and lower maternal age predicting greater decreases in partner aggression. While taking into account contextual and psychosocial risk factors, higher partner aggression measured at birth and a smaller decrease over time independently predicted higher levels of maternal harsh parenting at age 3 years. Initial level of partner aggression and change over time predicted child maladjustment indirectly (via maternal harsh parenting). The implications of understanding change in partner aggression over time as a path to harsh parenting and young children's maladjustment in the context of multiple risk factors are discussed. 相似文献
55.
ABSTRACT Abnormal shoulder and elbow muscle coactivation patterns, or muscle synergies, are commonly present following stroke and may arise through dysfunctional descending neural control from the cortex. The authors evaluated a novel technique for examining corticomotor movement encoding of the upper limb in three dimensions. A 6-degree-of-freedom loadcell recorded arm twitch responses in healthy adults following stimulation over the cortex or over Erb's point in the periphery. Stimuli were delivered while the arm generated a 5 N preload in each of the 6 axial directions. The initial force twitch response to stimulation was used to construct twitch direction vectors for each preload direction. General linear mixed model analyses were used to determine the influence of stimulation location, preload direction, posture, and stimulation intensity on twitch direction. Cortical stimulation gave rise to arm twitch responses that were predictably modified by preload direction. Peripheral stimulation elicited stereotypical twitches that were not influenced by preload. Our stimulation, recording, and analysis techniques were able to capture movement encoding of the upper limb in three dimensions. Such techniques could be utilized in the stroke population to determine and monitor the presence of upper limb synergies during muscle activation. 相似文献
56.
A fundamental step in learning words is the development of an association between a sound pattern and an element in the environment. Here we explore the nature of this associative ability in 12‐month‐olds, examining whether it is constrained to privilege particular word forms over others. Forty‐eight infants were presented with sets of novel English content‐like word–object pairings (e.g. fep) or novel English function‐like word–object (e.g. iv) pairings until they habituated. Results indicated that infants associated novel content‐like words, but not the novel function‐like words, with novel objects. These results demonstrate that the mechanism with which basic word–object associations are formed is remarkably sophisticated by the onset of productive language. That is, mere associative pairings are not sufficient to form mappings. Rather the system requires well‐formed noun‐like words to co‐occur with objects in order for the linkages to arise. 相似文献
57.
Studies of the natural language are a prime source of the Big-Five model, yet the factor analysis of a large, representative, and non-clustered set of English-language personality adjectives in a large sample has not yet been published. In order to test the hypothesis that finding the Big Five depends on biasing the variable selection with an investigator's preferred non-familiar terms, we present the factor analysis of 435 familiar adjectives in a combined sample (N=899) of 507 self- and 392 peer ratings. The five-factor solution reproduced the Big Five with high clarity, demonstrating generally very high correlations with Goldberg's adjective markers of the Big Five. The Intellect factor had a more moderate correlation, due to its de-emphasis of the creativity components of Factor V, a phenomenon that may occur commonly with the lexical Intellect factor. 相似文献
58.
Philosophical Studies - Pessimists think that there is something wrong with relying on deference for one’s moral beliefs—at least if one is morally mature. Call this no deference. They... 相似文献
59.
Science and Engineering Ethics - Prior to their announcement of the birth of gene-edited twins in China, Dr. He Jiankui and colleagues published a set of draft ethical principles for discussing the... 相似文献
60.