全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1230篇 |
免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
1259篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 37篇 |
2017年 | 28篇 |
2016年 | 27篇 |
2015年 | 25篇 |
2014年 | 23篇 |
2013年 | 122篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 47篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 48篇 |
2007年 | 50篇 |
2006年 | 40篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 29篇 |
2003年 | 32篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 17篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 19篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1971年 | 13篇 |
1970年 | 13篇 |
1969年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有1259条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
112.
P R Pietromonaco K S Rook M A Lewis 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1992,63(2):247-259
Conflicting views of depressives' interpersonal accuracy were addressed in an investigation of the accuracy of mild depressives (dysphorics) across differing social contexts. Women who were either friends or strangers and who were either similar or dissimilar in level of dysphoria conversed about 3 topics: a neutral topic, their own disclosure of a personal problem, and their partner's disclosure. Dysphorics were not more accurate in general than nondysphorics. After self-disclosure, all women more accurately detected sympathy from a similar stranger. After the partner's disclosure, women also better detected the mood of a similar partner. When interacting with nondysphoric strangers, however, dysphorics underestimated their partner's sympathy toward them. Implications for cognitive and interpersonal theories of depression and for depressives' relationships are discussed. 相似文献
113.
114.
115.
116.
In two experiments with paired rats, the effect of superimposing CS-US pairings on a baseline of shock-elicited aggression was studied. Baseline shocks (3.0 mA, 0.125-sec duration) occurred at a rate of 20 shocks per min throughout each session. In Experiment I, each independent group of two pairs of subjects received (in addition to baseline shocks) US shocks of 1.0, 3.0, or 5.0 mA and 5-sec duration, each shock signalled by a 1-min CS. At all three US intensities, aggression increased during the CS. In Experiment II, pairs of subjects received each unconditioned stimulus intensity in a within-subjects design. This procedure revealed a direct relationship between rate of responding and unconditioned shock intensity. 相似文献
117.
Lewis M Barker 《Learning and motivation》1976,7(2):265-273
The relative contributions of the duration, amount, and concentration of a taste CS were assessed in pairings with an ionizing radiation US in two experiments. Increasing the duration of the CS (0.1% saccharin) up to 9 min yielded increasingly better conditioning over a CS-US delay interval. When the concentration of the sacharin was increased, only a few seconds' exposure to the CS was necessary for near asymptotic conditioning. It was observed that the CS duration effect had not been identified earlier because most investigators utilize 10–20 min CS access periods. The role of CS duration in conditioning over a delay was discussed with reference to the probability of the memory of the CS being present at the time of the pairing with the US event. 相似文献
118.
Philip B. Clarke Amanda L. Giordano Craig S. Cashwell Todd F. Lewis 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2013,91(1):87-94
Spiritual bypass is the avoidance of underlying emotional issues by focusing solely on spiritual beliefs, practices, and experiences. Motivational interviewing (MI) is a client‐centered, compassionate approach to effectively addressing resistance among those who present with spiritual bypass. In this article, the authors provide background information on spiritual bypass and a rationale for adopting an MI approach to working with these clients. A case presentation illustrates 1 example of using MI with spiritual bypass. 相似文献
119.
Peter J. Lewis 《Synthese》2013,190(18):4009-4022
The Doomsday Argument and the Simulation Argument share certain structural features, and hence are often discussed together (Bostrom 2003, Are you living in a computer simulation, Philosophical Quarterly, 53:243–255; Aranyosi 2004, The Doomsday Simulation Argument. Or why isn’t the end nigh, and you’re not living in a simulation, http://philsci-archive.pitt.edu/190/; Richmond 2008, Doomsday, Bishop Ussher and simulated worlds, Ratio, 21:201–217; Bostrom and Kulczycki 2011 A patch for the Simulation Argument, Analysis, 71:54–61). Both are cases where reflecting on one’s location among a set of possibilities yields a counter-intuitive conclusion—in the first case that the end of humankind is closer than you initially thought, and in the second case that it is more likely than you initially thought that you are living in a computer simulation. Indeed, the two arguments do have some structural similarities. But there are also significant disanalogies between the two arguments, and I argue that these disanalogies mean that the Simulation Argument succeeds and the Doomsday Argument fails. 相似文献
120.
Panagiotis Matsangas Nita Lewis Shattuck Stephanie Brown 《Behavior research methods》2017,49(5):1792-1801
The 10-min psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) is a widely used tool to assess behavioral alertness. In operational environments, however, the 3-min version of the PVT is more practicable. Under controlled laboratory conditions, we compared performance on the 3-min PVT on a wrist-worn device with the 3-min PVT on a laptop computer. We conducted two repeated measures experiments (72 participants) to assess the effects of PVT device type (laptop or wrist-worn), backlight on the wrist-worn device (on or off), ambient light (low or normal levels), and interstimulus interval (ISI). First, we compared the wrist-worn PVT (ISI of 2–10 s) with the laptop-based PVT (ISI of 1–4 s). Participants performed six PVT trials counterbalanced for order of ambient light and device type. In both ambient-lighting conditions, the median differences in PVT metrics (reaction time, response speed, and percentage of 355-ms or 500-ms lapses combined with false starts) between the laptop and the wrist-worn PVT with the backlight on were less than 4.5 %. Specifically, the median difference in reaction times was less than 10 ms. However, the standard deviation in most metrics using the wrist-worn PVT was twice that of the laptop. In a second experiment, we compared the wrist-worn and laptop PVTs with the same ISIs (1–4 s) in normal light conditions. The results suggest that PVT data can be collected reliably in the field using the wrist-worn device with the backlight on and an ISI of 2–10 s. 相似文献