全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1410篇 |
免费 | 40篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1451篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 133篇 |
2012年 | 44篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 35篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 60篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 38篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 54篇 |
2003年 | 50篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 27篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 26篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 31篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 28篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 25篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1451条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
The counter model for word identification (Ratcliff & McKoon, 1997) has been challenged by recent empirical findings that performance on low-frequency words improves as the result of repetition of the words. We show that the model can accommodate this learning effect, and that it can do so without jeopardizing its explanations of the effects on word identification of a large number of other variables. 相似文献
62.
Christy A Poythress NG Boothroyd RA Petrila J Mehra S 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2005,23(2):227-243
Mental health courts have developed as one response to persons with mental illness who are involved with the criminal justice system. This study investigated the efficiency and safety goals of one such court in Broward County, FL. Mental health court (MHC) clients spent significantly fewer days in jail for the index arrest associated with study enrollment than a comparison group. MHC clients had similar survival time to re-arrest up to one year after study enrollment. MHC clients did not significantly differ from the comparison group in self-reported aggressive acts over an 8 month follow-up period, while they did self-report significantly fewer acts of violence than the comparison group at the 8 month follow-up. These findings suggest that some of the benefits associated with the MHC reported in prior studies were not achieved at the expense of efficiency and safety. 相似文献
63.
64.
Throughout the U.S., state laws require professionals who work with children to report cases of suspected child abuse to child protection services. Both practically and conceptually, however, significant problems arise from a lack of clarity regarding the threshold that has been set for reporting. Specifically, there is no consensus as to what constitutes reasonable suspicion, and little direction for how mandated reporters should gauge their legal and professional responsibilities when they harbor suspicion. In this paper we outline the context of the problem, discuss the nature and scope of its conceptual underpinnings, and offer recommendations for moving towards a concrete, practical solution. 相似文献
65.
We present a general sampling procedure to quantify model mimicry, defined as the ability of a model to account for data generated by a competing model. This sampling procedure, called the parametric bootstrap cross-fitting method (PBCM; cf. Williams (J. R. Statist. Soc. B 32 (1970) 350; Biometrics 26 (1970) 23)), generates distributions of differences in goodness-of-fit expected under each of the competing models. In the data informed version of the PBCM, the generating models have specific parameter values obtained by fitting the experimental data under consideration. The data informed difference distributions can be compared to the observed difference in goodness-of-fit to allow a quantification of model adequacy. In the data uninformed version of the PBCM, the generating models have a relatively broad range of parameter values based on prior knowledge. Application of both the data informed and the data uninformed PBCM is illustrated with several examples. 相似文献
66.
In all three studies described in this article, novices were trained to associate traits from the DSM-IV Cluster B personality disorder categories with four letters of the alphabet. Novices were also taught various gender associations (i.e., "base rate" information) with the categories (none, stereotype-consistent, or stereotype-inconsistent). Results showed that when no gender associations were taught (Study I), case gender did not affect ratings of case vignettes. When associations were consistent with social stereotypes (Study II), case gender influenced ratings in the expected direction. When associations were the opposite of social stereotypes (Study III), only the ratings for narcissistic and histrionic were affected. Across the three studies, these results suggested that subjects rated cases in accordance with known "base rates," but that the correspondence between base rates and stereotypic associations affected the consistency and magnitude of this base rate effect. 相似文献
67.
68.
69.
70.