全文获取类型
收费全文 | 217篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
235篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 14篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有235条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
T. P. Ho Patrick W. L. Leung Ernest S. L. Luk Eric Taylor John Bacon-Shone Felice Lieh Mak 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1996,24(4):417-431
The behavioral problems of Chinese children were examined in a questionnaire study of a representative sample of 3,069 seven-year-old Chinese schoolboys in Hong Kong using Rutter's questionnaires for completion by parents and teachers. Separate robust dimensions of hyperactivity, antisocial, and neurotic behaviors were evident. The correlations among different dimensions were similar to those reported in the West. Short-term longitudinal analysis suggested that these dimensions were stable over time and that antisocial behaviors might develop in hyperactive children. The finding highlights the necessity of differentiating the hyperactive domain from the antisocial one. A significant source effect was found in all three dimensions. Chinese schoolboys had nearly two times the level of questionnaire-rated hyperactivity compared with school boys in the West; but it is premature to conclude that hyperactivity is more common in Chinese schoolboys in Hong Kong. Cross-cultural differences in adults' expectations and tolerance remain a plausible explanation.This study was supported by the Croucher Foundation. We would like to thank the Education Department of Hong Kong for assistance in sampling. We are particularly grateful to the children, teachers, and parents who participated in this study. 相似文献
102.
Based on past findings that attributionally more complex people make less fundamental attribution error, it was hypothesized that they would show less punitiveness and racism. In a study of 102 undergraduates, this hypothesis received robust support. The effect of attributional complexity was significant in two different punitiveness measures, a rehabilitation support measure, and two different racism measures. Also, this effect still held when demographic variables, crime victimization history, and need for cognition were statistically controlled. Moreover, attributional complexity mediated the effect of need for cognition and gender on punitiveness and racism. Theoretical implications are discussed. 相似文献
103.
The facilitative effect of incorporating echolalia on teaching receptive naming of Chinese characters to children with autism was assessed. In Experiment 1, echoing the requested character name prior to the receptive naming task facilitated matching a character to its name. In addition, task performance was consistently maintained only when echolalia preceded the receptive manual response. Positive results from generalization tests suggested that learned responses occurred across various novel conditions. In Experiment 2, we examined the relation between task difficulty and speed of acquisition. All 3 participants achieved 100% correct responding in training, but learning less discriminable characters took more trials than learning more discriminable characters. These results provide support for incorporating echolalia as an educational tool within language instruction for some children with autism. 相似文献
104.
Preference for simple interval schedules of reinforcement in concurrent chains: Effects of segmentation ratio 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
A concurrent-chains procedure was used to examine pigeons' preferences between segmented and unsegmented terminal-link schedules of reinforcement. During the initial link, a pair of independent, concurrent variable-interval 60-s schedules was in effect. In the terminal link, reinforcement was provided by a chain fixed-interval fixed-interval schedule on one key and by a simple fixed-interval schedule with an equal interreinforcement interval in the other. The relative duration between the first and second components (segmentation ratio) in the terminal-link chained schedule was systematically varied while the terminal-link duration was kept constant at either 15 s or 30 s in two sets of conditions. With few exceptions, the simple schedule was preferred to the chained schedule. Furthermore, this preference was inversely related to the size of the segmentation ratio in the segmented schedule. When the segmentation ratio was smaller than 1:1, preference was more extreme for a 30-s condition than for a 15-s condition. However, preference decreased more rapidly in conditions with the longer terminal-link duration when the ratio increased. Taken together, these results were consistent with previous findings concerning the effect of the terminal-link duration on choice between segmented and unsegmented schedules. In addition, the data suggested that segmentation ratio in a segmented schedule constitutes another potent factor influencing preference for the unsegmented schedule. 相似文献
105.
Elderly persons living in public housing experience a variety of problems. Counselors can help these residents by assisting them to cope better with their environment and personal relationships. This article discusses some specific problems, concerns, and approaches for counselors working with the elderly. 相似文献
106.
James H. Liu Robert Jiqi Zhang Angela K.-Y. Leung Homero Gil de Zúñiga Cecilia Gastardo-Conaco Vadym Vasiutynskyi Larissa Kus-Harbord 《Political psychology》2020,41(4):661-678
Psychology has begun contributing to social theory by providing empirical measures of actually existing cosmopolitanism that complements more purely theoretical conceptions of the construct common in philosophy and sociology. Drawing from two waves of research on representative adult samples from 19 countries (N = 8740), metric invariance was found for the three factors of cosmopolitan orientation (COS): cultural openness (CO), global prosociality (GP), and respect for cultural diversity (RCD). In terms of etiology, among Wave 1 measures, the personality factor of agreeableness was the best predictor of the cosmopolitan factors of GP and RCD at Wave 2, whereas openness of personality best predicted CO. Wave 1 measures of education, political liberalism, and self-reported social status independently also explained a small amount of variance in COS. Functionally, COS was shown to predict less prejudice against immigrants, and more support for global civil society, even after controlling for social dominance orientation. All three COS factors independently predicted better attitudes towards immigrants. GP was the best predictor of trust in the United Nations, whereas RCD was the best predictor of support for environmental protection. The three-factor model of COS appears well-calibrated for assessing actually existing cosmopolitanism across cultures. 相似文献
107.
Beatriz Calvo-Merino Shantel Ehrenberg Delia Leung Patrick Haggard 《Psychological research》2010,74(4):400-406
Biological motion perception is influenced by observers’ familiarity with the observed action. Here, we used classical dance
as a means to investigate how visual and motor experience modulates perceptual mechanism for configural processing of actions.
Although some ballet moves are performed by only one gender, male and female dancers train together and acquire visual knowledge
of all ballet moves. Twenty-four expert ballet dancers (12 female) and matched non-expert participants viewed pairs of upright
and inverted point light female and common dance movements. Visual discrimination between different exemplars of the same
movement presented upright was significantly better in experts than controls, whilst no differences were found when the same
stimuli were presented upside down. These results suggest expertise influences configural action processing. Within the expert
group, effects were stronger for female participants than for males, whilst no differences were found between movement types.
This observer gender effect could suggest an additional role for motor familiarity in action perception, over and above the
visual experience. Our results are consistent with a specific motor contribution to configural processing of action. 相似文献
108.
Leon Mann Mark Radford Paul Burnett Steve Ford Michael Bond Kwok Leung Hiyoshi Nakamura Graham Vaughan Kuo-Shu Yang 《International journal of psychology》1998,33(5):325-335
The Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire (Mann, Burnett, Radford, & Ford, 1997) measures self-reported decision-making coping patterns. The questionnaire was administered to samples of University students in the US (N = 475), Australia (N = 262), New Zealand (N = 260), Japan (N = 359), Hong Kong (N = 281), and Taiwan (N = 414). As predicted, students from the three Western, individualistic cultures (US, Australia, and New Zealand) were more confident of their decision-making ability than students from the three East Asian, group-oriented cultures (Japan, Hong Kong, Taiwan). No cross-cultural differences were found in scores on decision vigilance (a careful decision-making style). However, compared with Western students, the Asian students tended to score higher on buck-passing and procrastination (avoidant styles of decision making) as well as hypervigilance (a panicky style of decision making). Japanese students scored lowest on decision self-esteem and highest on procrastination and hypervigilance. It was argued that the conflict model and its attendant coping patterns is relevant for describing and comparing decision making in both Western and Asian cultures. 相似文献
109.
Leung JP 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1993,59(2):401-410
College students responded for monetary rewards in two experiments on choice between differentially segmented aperiodic schedules of reinforcement. On a microcomputer, the concurrent chains were simulated as an air-defense video game in which subjects used two radars for detecting and destroying enemy aircraft. To earn more cash-exchangeable points, subjects had to shoot down as many planes as possible within a given period of time. For both experiments, access to one of two radar systems (terminal link) was controlled by a pair of independent concurrent variable-interval 60-s schedules (initial link) with a 4-s changeover delay always in effect. In Experiment 1, the appearance of an enemy aircraft in the terminal link was determined by a variable-interval (15 s or 60 s) schedule or a two-component chained variable-interval schedule of equal duration. Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 except for the segmented schedule, which had three components. Subjects preferred the unsegmented schedule over its segmented counterpart in the conditions with variable-interval 60 s, and preference tended to be more pronounced with more components in the segmented schedule. These findings are compatible with those from previous studies of periodic and aperiodic schedules with pigeons or humans as subjects. 相似文献
110.
Applied Research in Quality of Life - This exploratory study examines the effects of LinkedIn users’ personality traits, use intensity, and LinkedIn feature usage patterns on their perceived... 相似文献