首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   54343篇
  免费   1520篇
  国内免费   25篇
  2020年   416篇
  2019年   540篇
  2018年   4027篇
  2017年   3353篇
  2016年   2928篇
  2015年   720篇
  2014年   720篇
  2013年   2929篇
  2012年   1727篇
  2011年   3469篇
  2010年   3114篇
  2009年   2197篇
  2008年   2650篇
  2007年   3090篇
  2006年   933篇
  2005年   983篇
  2004年   923篇
  2003年   835篇
  2002年   773篇
  2001年   1162篇
  2000年   1192篇
  1999年   846篇
  1998年   384篇
  1997年   358篇
  1996年   324篇
  1995年   332篇
  1992年   671篇
  1991年   619篇
  1990年   612篇
  1989年   590篇
  1988年   587篇
  1987年   504篇
  1986年   514篇
  1985年   564篇
  1984年   459篇
  1983年   416篇
  1982年   324篇
  1979年   490篇
  1978年   337篇
  1976年   326篇
  1975年   370篇
  1974年   441篇
  1973年   469篇
  1972年   408篇
  1971年   351篇
  1970年   333篇
  1969年   344篇
  1968年   429篇
  1967年   381篇
  1966年   368篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
931.
932.
933.
934.
935.
936.
937.
Two doses diethylpropion, one dose pipradrol, one dose amobarbital and placebo were administered to 116 subjects, who rated their state on twenty variables. The variables were intercorrelated under pipradrol and pentymal. The correlation matrices were subjected to factor analysis. Four factors were found in both studies: Happiness, Alertness, Relaxation, and Flight of thoughts. There is some agreement between the two factor analyses. There were few significant effects of the drugs on factor scores, but the structure is quite unambiguous: pipradrol and diethylpropion are stimulating, and amobarbital tranquilizing.  相似文献   
938.
Response latencies in naming objects   总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16  
After some preliminary analysis of what is involved in naming objects, in which the possible role of classificatory systems in the memory store is discussed, it is shown experimentally that there are consistent differences between the times taken to respond to presented objects by uttering their names, variations between the performances of different individuals being outweighed by variations due to the different objects. Moreover, there is a high consistency between different individuals as to the ordering of objects in respect of their naming latencies. It is further shown that a high correlation exists between the time taken to name an object and the frequency with which its name occurs in the language as a whole, as estimated in the Thorndike-Lorge Word List. Some implications of these findings are discussed, especially with reference to possible mechanisms by which presented objects are visually identified, and the appropriate names retrieved from the “word-store.”  相似文献   
939.
Memory performance awareness in younger and older adults   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Reports on the course of memory-monitoring skills across adulthood are discrepant in conclusions and limited in scope. The purpose of this study was to build a large data base (3 samples and 7 different tasks) to assess performance awareness. Younger (19-41 years) and older (59-93 years) Ss estimated performance either before (i.e., predictions) or after (i.e., postdictions) completing each task. Predictions were less accurate than postdictions at both age levels, suggesting Ss monitored performance during the study-test cycle. Overall, the data suggested no consistent age effects in performance awareness: Age differences in monitoring occurred only in predictions and only for some tasks. Between-tasks differences in age effects could not be attributed to a single mediating mechanism like those suggested in previous reports. Why previous research has produced conflicting conclusions about metacognitive development in adulthood is discussed in light of these data.  相似文献   
940.
The usual superiority in frequency-of-occurrence judgments of younger vs. older subjects was hypothesized to result from greater strategic encoding of the materials conveying frequency information. A subject-paced, visual search task was designed to control nontarget word encoding. Relative frequency judgments for the nontarget word pairs were equally accurate for younger and older subjects, and performance of both groups was above chance. Results suggest that strategic cover-task encoding can induce age differences in incidental frequency processing. Consistent with a nonoptimal as opposed to an optimal view, automatic processes require only minimal capacity for above-chance performance, but additional strategic resources can increase performance. When such additional resources are used more by younger than by older subjects, the former are better in frequency performance.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号