全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1037篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1056篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 28篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 26篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 144篇 |
2012年 | 26篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 35篇 |
2007年 | 31篇 |
2006年 | 39篇 |
2005年 | 31篇 |
2004年 | 35篇 |
2003年 | 35篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1964年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1056条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
Leslie Greenberg 《Psychoanalytic Dialogues》2014,24(4):427-432
In this comment I applaud Goldner’s ability to capture couple experience and dynamics while emphasizing that coupling involves more than attachment. In my view, couple conflict and therapy involves attending not only to attachment, which involves closeness and the threat of separation, but also to issues of identity, self-definition, and shame. I point out how some of the interactions and dynamics in the case discussed involve a threat to identity through invalidation more than to attachment by abandonment. As Goldner notes, mutual recognition between people is the cornerstone of the relational ideal, and it is for recognition through being seen, and validated for which Bill and Jane fight so hard, not closeness. In my view not everything in couples is about regulating attachment; rather everything is probably about regulating affect. Attachment is one, but only one, of the ways couples regulate their affect. 相似文献
182.
183.
In Drosophila, calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII) activity is crucial in associative courtship conditioning for both memory formation and suppression of courtship during training with a mated female. We have previously shown that increasing levels of constitutively active CaMKII, but not calcium-dependent CaMKII, in a subset of neurons can decrease the initial level of courtship and enhance the rate of suppression of courtship in response to a mated female. In this study, we demonstrate that a subpopulation of noncholinergic, nondopaminergic, non-GABAergic neurons can cause CaMKII-dependent reductions in initial courtship, but only cholinergic neurons enhance training-dependent suppression. These data suggest that processing of pheromonal signals in two subpopulations of neurons, likely antennal lobe projection neurons, is critical for behavioral plasticity. 相似文献
184.
According to Beck's (1988) clinical observations, patients with panic disorder describe a fixation on their distressing physical and psychological symptoms and an inability to access corrective information during panic attacks. The present study sought to evaluate empirically the notion that attentional fixation is characteristic of these patients. A subset of panic patients participating in 3 cognitive therapy clinical trials completed the Attentional Fixation Questionnaire (AFQ) at intake, during treatment and at termination. The AFQ had adequate internal consistency at all time intervals, and it correlated positively with measures of depression, anxiety and distorted cognitive content. At termination, the AFQ total score and nearly all single items decreased significantly, and patients who continued to meet diagnostic criteria for panic disorder scored higher on most items than patients who no longer met diagnostic criteria for panic disorder. These preliminary data suggest that attentional fixation is an important dimension of cognition relevant to panic disorder. 相似文献
185.
This study tested the hypotheses that people had a bias for drawing agents on the left of a picture when given a verb stimulus targeting an active or passive event (e.g., "kicked" or "is kicked") and that orthographic directionality would influence the way events were illustrated. Monolingual English speakers, who read and write left-to-right, and Arabic speakers, who read and write right-to-left, drew agents and patients in response to verb stimuli. We found no significant orthographic directionality effects and no preference for positioning agents on the left of pictures in either group or sentence type. Instead, participants drew agents on the right regardless of language or sentence type, and this was exaggerated in English speakers illustrating passive verbs. These findings support the existence of a preference for placing agents in the right hemispace that may result from asymmetrical hemispheric (i.e., left>right) activation induced by language processing. Our results are consistent with findings that people prefer pictures in which focus is on the right, a preference strongest in pictures with no implicit directionality of movement. This suggests that the methodology of the current study encouraged a static rather than dynamic interpretation of the verb in most participants. 相似文献
186.
When young children observe pretend-play, do they interpret it simply as a type of behavior, or do they infer the underlying mental state that gives the behavior meaning? This is a long-standing question with deep implications for how “theory on mind” develops. The two leading accounts of shared pretense give opposing answers. The behavioral theory proposes that children represent pretense as a form of behavior (behaving in a way that would be appropriate if P); the metarepresentational theory argues that children instead represent pretense via the early concept PRETEND. A test between these accounts is provided by children’s understanding of pretend sounds and speech. We report the first experiments directly investigating this understanding. In three experiments, 2- and 3-year-olds’ listened to requests that were either spoken normally, or with the pretense that a teddy bear was uttering them. To correctly fulfill the requests, children had to represent the normal utterance as the experimenter’s, and the pretend utterances as the bear’s. Children succeeded at both ages, suggesting that they can represent pretend speech (the requests) as coming from counterfactual sources (the bear rather than the experimenter). We argue that this is readily explained by the metarepresentational theory, but harder to explain if children are behaviorists about pretense. 相似文献
187.
David Dibattista Laurie Hollis-walker Leslie Hague 《The Journal of general psychology》2013,140(4):446-461
In Experiment 1, golden hamsters were injected with either 0.9% saline or the nausea-inducing agent, lithium chloride (LiCL), immediately after consuming a flavored diet that was either novel or familiar. The LiCl-induced aversion was strong in hamsters for which the flavored diet was novel, but no significant aversion was observed in hamsters that were familiar with the flavored diet. In Experiment 2, the strength of the LiCl-induced aversion was related inversely to the amount of conditioned-stimulus (CS) preexposure and directly to the duration of the preexposure-conditioning interval. Thus, although some previous researchers have suggested that hamsters may not demonstrate the CS-preexposure effect in a conditioned taste-aversion paradigm, they clearly did so under the conditions of the present experiments, and moreover, the characteristics of the CS-preexposure effect in hamsters were generally similar to those observed in rats. 相似文献
188.
W. Avery Carl A. Ridley Leigh A. Leslie Tom Milholland 《The American journal of family therapy》2013,41(3):23-30
Abstract The purpose of the present study was to assess the short-term and longterm effectiveness of relationship enhancement on the self-disclosure and empathy skills of premarital dating couples. Nineteen couples were given the Guerney Relationship Enhancement Program over an eight week period, while 18 other couples were given a lecture/discussion program on relationship development. Behavioral assessments of self-disclosure and empathy skill levels were taken prior to training, immediately following training, and six months after training. Results indicated that from both pretest to posttest and pretest to follow-up, the relationship enhancement couples, relative to the lecture/discussion couples, demonstrated a significant increase in both self-disclosure and empathy skill levels. Discussion centers on the implications of the durability of the skills learned as well as on suggestions for future research on skill generalization to social interactions with others. 相似文献
189.
AbstractThere have been numerous studies of the impact of sex-role stereotyping upon the preferences for job rewards and the difference between men and women. This study tests for such differences in a career field where men and women compete equally—commissioned residential real estate sales. Several new hypotheses were tested to include not only differences in desirability for job rewards, but also the expectancy of success in achieving them. There was little evidence of any differences between men and women in this career field resulting from a sex-role impact. 相似文献
190.
Maria M. Steenkamp William P. Nash Leslie Lebowitz Brett T. Litz 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2013,20(4):471-475
We comment on Smith, Duax, and Rauch’s (2013--this issue) explication of their approach to treating perpetration-related guilt and shame using prolonged exposure (PE) therapy, with the aim of promoting a discourse about the mechanisms, techniques, and assumptions that underlie the treatment of moral injury in veterans and service members. We first discuss the theoretical foundation underlying PE and consider the extent to which it accounts for the phenomenology of moral injury. We then examine the treatment strategies used in the PE approach and the mechanisms by which these techniques ameliorate perpetration-related guilt and shame. We also briefly highlight points of similarity and contrast between PE and adaptive disclosure, a brief cognitive behavioral intervention targeting combat-related moral injury and traumatic loss. 相似文献