全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1541篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
1566篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 21篇 |
2023年 | 16篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 27篇 |
2020年 | 53篇 |
2019年 | 38篇 |
2018年 | 67篇 |
2017年 | 60篇 |
2016年 | 72篇 |
2015年 | 62篇 |
2014年 | 49篇 |
2013年 | 230篇 |
2012年 | 73篇 |
2011年 | 87篇 |
2010年 | 61篇 |
2009年 | 56篇 |
2008年 | 95篇 |
2007年 | 71篇 |
2006年 | 51篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 29篇 |
2003年 | 43篇 |
2002年 | 41篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1938年 | 3篇 |
1937年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1566条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Pekka Väyrynen 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2004,7(1):53-79
This paper addresses a recent suggestion that moral particularists can extend their view to countenance default reasons (at
a first stab, reasons that are pro tanto unless undermined) by relying on certain background expectations of normality. I
first argue that normality must be understood non-extensionally. Thus if default reasons rest on normality claims, those claims
won't bestow upon default reasons any definite degree of extensional generality. Their generality depends rather on the contingent
distributional aspects of the world, which no theory of reasons should purport to settle. Appeals to default reasons cannot
therefore uniquely support particularism. But this argument also implies that if moral generalism entailed that moral reasons
by necessity have invariant valence (in the natural extensional sense), it would be a non-starter. Since generalism is not
a non-starter, my argument forces us to rethink the parameters of the generalism-particularism debate. Here I propose to clarify
the debate by focusing on its modal rather than extensional aspects. In closing, I outline the sort of generalism that I think is motivated by my discussion,
and then articulate some worries this view raises about the theoretical usefulness of the label ‘default reason’. 相似文献
72.
Neuhäuser M 《Perceptual and motor skills》2005,101(2):510-514
Baumgartner, Weiss, and Schindler (1998) introduced a novel non-parametric test for the two-sample comparison that is superior to commonly used tests such as the Wilcoxon rank-sum test. A modification of the novel test statistic can be used for one-sided comparisons based on ordinal data. Such comparisons frequently occur in psychological research, and the Wilcoxon test is often recommended for their analysis. Here, the two tests were compared in a simulation study. According to this study the tests have a similar type I error rate, but the modified Baumgartner-Weiss-Schindler test is more powerful than the Wilcoxon test. 相似文献
73.
Five-year changes in episodic and semantic memory were examined in a sample of 829 participants (35-80 years). A cohort-matched sample (N=967) was assessed to control for practice effects. For episodic memory, cross-sectional analyses indicated gradual age-related decrements, whereas the longitudinal data revealed no decrements before age 60, even when practice effects were adjusted for. Longitudinally, semantic memory showed minor increments until age 55, with smaller decrements in old age as compared with episodic memory. Cohort differences in educational attainment appear to account for the discrepancies between cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Collectively, the results show that age trajectories for episodic and semantic memory differ and underscore the need to control for cohort and retest effects in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, respectively. 相似文献
74.
Recent findings are reviewed indicating that upper alpha oscillations - when analyzed with appropriate time/ frequency resolution - show a similar physiological reactivity as theta in working memory tasks. Comparable to theta, a load dependent increase in power can be observed during retention and increased evoked activity during retrieval. During retrieval attempts theta behaves like a traveling wave spreading from anterior to posterior sites. During actual retrieval, however, evoked upper alpha becomes transiently nested in theta. We suggest that theta reflects working memory functions whereas upper alpha may be important for the reactivation of long-term memory codes in short-term memory. 相似文献
75.
Närhi V Ahonen T Aro M Leppäsaari T Korhonen TT Tolvanen A Lyytinen H 《Brain and language》2005,92(1):45-57
We report two studies on rapid serial naming (RSN). Study 1 addressed the relations among RSN tasks comprising different stimuli. Separate components for RSN of alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric stimuli, as well as for tasks in which the stimuli alternated between categories were identified. In Study 2, phonological skills, processing speed, motor dexterity, and verbal fluency were found to explain RSN performance. The studies indicate: (1) that RSN tasks vary in their properties according to the stimuli used and according to the way the tasks are arranged, and (2) that RSN tasks are multi-componented. 相似文献
76.
Rämä P 《Cognitive processing》2008,9(1):29-34
Visual system has been proposed to be divided into two, the ventral and dorsal, processing streams. The ventral pathway is
thought to be involved in object identification whereas the dorsal pathway processes information regarding the spatial locations
of objects and the spatial relationships among objects. Several studies on working memory (WM) processing have further suggested
that there is a dissociable domain-dependent functional organization within the prefrontal cortex for processing of spatial
and nonspatial visual information. Also the auditory system is proposed to be organized into two domain-specific processing
streams, similar to that seen in the visual system. Recent studies on auditory WM have further suggested that maintenance
of nonspatial and spatial auditory information activates a distributed neural network including temporal, parietal, and frontal
regions but the magnitude of activation within these activated areas shows a different functional topography depending on
the type of information being maintained. The dorsal prefrontal cortex, specifically an area of the superior frontal sulcus
(SFS), has been shown to exhibit greater activity for spatial than for nonspatial auditory tasks. Conversely, ventral frontal
regions have been shown to be more recruited by nonspatial than by spatial auditory tasks. It has also been shown that the
magnitude of this dissociation is dependent on the cognitive operations required during WM processing. Moreover, there is
evidence that within the nonspatial domain in the ventral prefrontal cortex, there is an across-modality dissociation during
maintenance of visual and auditory information. Taken together, human neuroimaging results on both visual and auditory sensory
systems support the idea that the prefrontal cortex is organized according to the type of information being maintained in
WM. 相似文献
77.
Camilla Jalling Maria Bodin Anders Romelsjö Håkan Källmén Natalie Durbeej Anders Tengström 《Journal of child and family studies》2016,25(3):811-826
Two theoretically based parent training programs, delivered in real-world settings by the social services, were examined in this randomized controlled trial for effectiveness in reducing adolescents’ antisocial behavior and substance use. Two hundred and thirty-seven (237) adolescents in ages between 12 and 18 and their parents were assigned to one of two programs or to a wait-list control condition. The programs were the nine weekly group sessions program Comet 12–18 (Swedish Parent Management Training Program) and the six weekly ParentSteps (Swedish shortened version by Strengthening Families Program 10–14). Outcome measures were antisocial behavior, substance use, and delinquency, and psychosocial dysfunction. Data based on adolescents’ and parents’ ratings of the adolescents’ problem behavior at baseline and 6 months later were analyzed with repeated measures ANVOA, Logistic regression, and Kruskal–Wallis H test. The results showed that parents’ ratings of adolescents’ antisocial behaviors decreased significantly over time, but no time by group effect emerged. No program effects were found in the adolescents’ self-reported antisocial behavior, delinquency, or psychosocial functioning. A threefold risk of illicit drug use was found in both intervention groups. The results suggest that neither Comet nor ParentSteps had beneficial effects on adolescent’s antisocial or delinquent behavior, or on alcohol use. The only significant group difference found was a threefold risk of drug use in the intervention adolescents at follow-up, but for several reasons this finding should be interpreted with caution. Trial registration number: ISRCTN76141538. 相似文献
78.
Group agents are able to act but are not literally agents. Some group agents, e.g., we-mode groups and corporations, can, however, be regarded as functional group agents that do not have “intrinsic” mental states and phenomenal features comparable to what their individual members on biological and psychological grounds have. But they can have “extrinsic” mental states, states collectively attributed to them—primarily by their members. In this paper, we discuss the responsibility of such group agents. We defend the view that if the group members have accepted the group agent’s (attributed) attitudes and are committed to them, we can favorably compare the situation with the case of individual human agents and a group agent can be regarded as morally responsible for its intentional activities. 相似文献
79.
The present experiment investigated whether increased media exposure could lead to an increase in memory distortions regarding a traumatic public event: the explosion of the No. 30 bus in Tavistock Square, London on 7 July 2005. A total of 150 Swedish and 150 UK participants completed a series of questionnaires about their memory of either (i) the aftermath of the explosion, (ii) a non-existent computerised reconstruction of the moment of the explosion, or (iii) non-existent closed circuit television footage of the moment of the explosion. In line with the availability heuristic, U.K. participants were more likely than Swedish participants to claim to have seen all three types of footage. Furthermore, a subsample of U.K. participants who appeared to have developed false "memories" of seeing the No. 30 bus explode scored significantly higher on measures of dissociation and fantasy proneness than participants who did not develop false "memories". This experiment provides further support for the role of imaginative processes in the development of false memories. 相似文献
80.
Emotional-neutral pairs of visual scenes were presented peripherally (with their inner edges 5.2 degrees away from fixation) as primes for 150 to 900 ms, followed by a centrally presented recognition probe scene, which was either identical in specific content to one of the primes or related in general content and affective valence. Results indicated that (a) if no foveal fixations on the primes were allowed, the false alarm rate for emotional probes was increased; (b) hit rate and sensitivity (A') were higher for emotional than for neutral probes only when a fixation was possible on only one prime; and (c) emotional scenes were more likely to attract the first fixation than neutral scenes. It is concluded that the specific content of emotional or neutral scenes is not processed in peripheral vision. Nevertheless, a coarse impression of emotional scenes may be extracted, which then leads to selective attentional orienting or--in the absence of overt attention--causes false alarms for related probes. 相似文献