全文获取类型
收费全文 | 423篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
448篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 49篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1958年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有448条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Behavioral aftereffects of reinforcement and its omission as a function of reinforcement magnitude 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Rats responded on a multiple fixed-interval fixed-interval schedule of reinforcement. Each complete cycle of the multiple schedule was separated from the next by a relatively long period of timeout from all schedule contingencies. A response at the end of the second component of each cycle was always reinforced with an invariant reinforcement magnitude, while reinforcement magnitude and reinforcement omission were systematically varied in the first component. Response rate in the first component was a monotonic function of reinforcement magnitude in that component. These changes in response rate in the first component did not affect response rate in the second component. When reinforcement was omitted on 50% of occasions in the first component, following reinforcement there was a reduction in response rate in the second component that was monotonically related to reinforcement magnitude. Following reinforcement omission there was an increase in response rate in the second component that was unrelated to reinforcement magnitude. When reinforcement was omitted on 100% of occasions in the first component, behavioral contrast was observed. 相似文献
92.
93.
The Stroop color-word test: a review 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
94.
Jesper Jensen 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1960,1(1):157-162
The interpretation of functional fixedness as a fixation of behavior or inhibiting of behavior tendencies is challenged. As an alternative is proposed 'perceptual emphasis' in which the effect of past experience with the use of objects is viewed in agreement with van de Geer as a change in the perception of the objects. Some recent experiments are reviewed in the light of this phenomenological interpretation. 相似文献
95.
Arthur R. Jensen 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1965,17(4):315-322
An adjacency effect was demonstrated at a high level of significance in the free recall, by 123 subjects, of a list of 40 high-frequency nouns presented in varying order on successive trials. The phenomenon referred to as the adjacency effect consists of the fact that when a subject is given repeated trials of study and free recall of a list of words (always presented in a different order), the probability of recalling a given item is greater when the item is presented temporally adjacent to an item which is already learned (as evidenced by recall on the previous trial) than when the item stands temporally between other items which are not yet learned. The enhancement of recall is greater when the item is presented between two previously learned items. The implications of the adjacency effect for verbal learning theory, particularly for the serial-position effect in serial learning and the concepts of interference and neural consolidation, are discussed. 相似文献
96.
Arthur R. Jensen 《Intelligence》1985,9(3):285-289
Humphreys's test of Spearman's hypothesis (viz., that the size of the standardized black-white differences on various psychometric tests is positively related to the tests' loadings on g, the general intelligence factor) is methodologically weak. It is based on comparison of a fairly representative sample of the black population of U.S. school children with a highly selected sample of the white school population, representing the lowest 15 to 20% of whites in socio-economic status (SES). A fair test of the hypothesis requires that the black and white samples should not be selected on any g-correlated variable, such as SES. Selection attenuates and distorts the relationship between tests' g loadings and the magnitude of the standardized mean black-white differences on the tests. Other unorthodox conditions in Humphreys's study, such as performing factor analysis on the test-score means of various arbitrary demographic groups instead of on individual test scores inflates tests' g loadings and biases the test of Spearman's hypothesis by restriction of reliable variance in g loadings. Humphreys's study cannot be considered a proper replication of Jensen's examination of Spearman's hypothesis in 11 different studies comprising 74 different tests, which consistently bears out Spearman's hypothesis. 相似文献
97.
Young women's motivations for two sex-role options were predicted with scores describing perceived rewards/costs of parenting. Using a sequence of multiple regression analyses it was found that the reward-cost perceptions of parenting and work were predictors of the two motivational options. Individual rewards/costs of parenting and work items were better predictors than summed items. It was found that a preference for the career choice was produced by not viewing parenting as rewarding. The preference for parenting choice was influenced most by perceiving parenting as rewarding. The perceived cost and rewards of work were not predictors of a career choice. The items found to be the best predictors for either the parenting or career choice all dealt with perceptions of parenting. It was concluded that these parenting perceptions are the key elements for young women's role motivations. 相似文献
98.
99.
The Young and the Reckless: Adolescent Reckless Behavior 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Jeffrey Arnett 《Current directions in psychological science》1995,4(3):67-70
100.
Benjamin B. Lahey Sherryl H. Goodman Irwin D. Waldman Hector Bird Glorisa Canino Peter Jensen Darrel Regier Philip J. Leaf Rachel Gordon Brooks Applegate 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1999,27(4):247-260
In a cross-sectional household sample of 9-through 17-year-old youths from 4 U.S. communities, youths with earlier ages of onset of conduct problems engaged in more conduct problems than youths with later ages of onset when current age and gender were controlled. Specifically, youths with earlier ages of onset were more likely to engage in several types of physical aggression, frequent lying, theft, and vandalism and were less likely to engage in only truancy. There also was an inverse relation between age of onset and level of functional impairment, mental health service use, and meeting diagnostic criteria for conduct disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder. Within the limits of cross-sectional data, these results support the hypothesis that key aspects of the heterogeneity of conduct problems among youths are related to the age of onset of conduct problems. 相似文献