全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3956篇 |
免费 | 51篇 |
专业分类
4007篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 34篇 |
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 41篇 |
2021年 | 73篇 |
2020年 | 84篇 |
2019年 | 115篇 |
2018年 | 149篇 |
2017年 | 149篇 |
2016年 | 165篇 |
2015年 | 116篇 |
2014年 | 134篇 |
2013年 | 455篇 |
2012年 | 203篇 |
2011年 | 233篇 |
2010年 | 133篇 |
2009年 | 135篇 |
2008年 | 185篇 |
2007年 | 150篇 |
2006年 | 133篇 |
2005年 | 101篇 |
2004年 | 122篇 |
2003年 | 125篇 |
2002年 | 97篇 |
2001年 | 75篇 |
2000年 | 82篇 |
1999年 | 56篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 21篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 38篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 46篇 |
1989年 | 43篇 |
1988年 | 37篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 30篇 |
1985年 | 29篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
1938年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有4007条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
291.
Rob van Someren Greve 《Philosophical Studies》2014,168(1):167-177
Some moral theories, such as objective forms of consequentialism, seem to fail to be practically useful: they are of little to no help in trying to decide what to do. Even if we do not think this constitutes a fatal flaw in such theories, we may nonetheless agree that being practically useful does make a moral theory a better theory, or so some have suggested. In this paper, I assess whether the uncontroversial respect in which a moral theory can be claimed to be better if it is practically useful can provide a ground worth taking into account for believing one theory rather than another. I argue that this is not the case. The upshot is that if there is a sound objection to theories such as objective consequentialism that is based on considerations of practical usefulness, the objection requires that it is established that the truth about what we morally ought to do cannot be epistemically inaccessible to us. The value of practical usefulness has no bearing on the issue. 相似文献
292.
Witt et al. (2008) have recently shown that golfers who putt with more success perceive the hole to be bigger than golfers who putt with less success. In three experiments, we systematically examined whether this phenomenon, labelled action-specific perception, depends on directing visual attention towards the action target. In Experiment 1 we replicated previously reported action-specific effects on perception in golf putting. In 3 and 4 we directly assessed whether action-specific effects on perception in golf putting are dependent on focusing visual attention on the target. To this end, the participants performed the putting task while visual attention towards the target was either completely withheld (Experiment 2) or divided over the target and other task-relevant objects (Experiment 3). No action-specific effects were found when visual attention towards the action target was occluded or partially diverted from the target. Together, our results provide evidence to suggest that focusing visual attention on the target while performing the action is a prerequisite for the emergence of action-specific perception. 相似文献
293.
The effect of local kinematic changes on anticipation judgments 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Cañal-Bruland R van Ginneken WF van der Meer BR Williams AM 《Human movement science》2011,30(3):495-503
Skilled tennis players rely on a more global than local perceptual strategy when anticipating an opponent’s shot direction in tennis (Williams, Huys, Cañal-Bruland, & Hagemann, 2009). Global perceptual strategy refers to the pick-up of dynamic information across different body areas rather than from a single source such as the arm or racket. We extend previous work by examining the spatiotemporal characteristics of visual information pick-up when anticipating shot direction in tennis. We perturbed information from various body locations by interchanging the dynamics of selected areas with those from strokes played to the opposite side of the court. In addition, we presented each manipulation under four different temporal occlusion conditions (from 240 ms prior to racket-ball contact) to examine the time course of information extraction. Skilled tennis players outperformed less skilled counterparts in predicting shot direction, even at early phases of the opponent’s motion. Moreover, skilled players showed significant decrements in performance when the arms and racket were perturbed, highlighting the relevance of distal information when anticipating opponents’ intentions. 相似文献
294.
There is considerable evidence that children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders (ASD) are at increased risk
of anxiety and anxiety disorders. However, it is less clear which of the specific DSM-IV anxiety disorders occur most in this
population. The present study used meta-analytic techniques to help clarify this issue. A systematic review of the literature
identified 31 studies involving 2,121 young people (aged <18 years) with ASD, and where the presence of anxiety disorder was
assessed using standardized questionnaires or diagnostic interviews. Across studies, 39.6% of young people with ASD had at
least one comorbid DSM-IV anxiety disorder, the most frequent being specific phobia (29.8%) followed by OCD (17.4%) and social
anxiety disorder (16.6%). Associations were found between the specific anxiety disorders and ASD subtype, age, IQ, and assessment
method (questionnaire versus interview). Implications for the identification and treatment of anxiety in young people with
ASD are discussed. 相似文献
295.
Robots are increasingly expected to perform tasks in complex environments. To this end, engineers provide them with processing
architectures that are based on models of human information processing. In contrast to traditional models, where information
processing is typically set up in stages (i.e., from perception to cognition to action), it is increasingly acknowledged by
psychologists and robot engineers that perception and action are parts of an interactive and integrated process. In this paper,
we present HiTEC, a novel computational (cognitive) model that allows for direct interaction between perception and action
as well as for cognitive control, demonstrated by task-related attentional influences. Simulation results show that key behavioral
studies can be readily replicated. Three processing aspects of HiTEC are stressed for their importance for cognitive robotics:
(1) ideomotor learning of action control, (2) the influence of task context and attention on perception, action planning,
and learning, and (3) the interaction between perception and action planning. Implications for the design of cognitive robotics
are discussed. 相似文献
296.
Neumann A van Lier PA Frijns T Meeus W Koot HM 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2011,39(5):657-669
This study examined the role of the level and variability of happiness, anger, anxiety, and sadness in the development of
adolescent-reported anxiety disorder symptoms, depressive symptoms, and aggressive behavior in 452 adolescents (250 male)
followed from age 13 to 14. Level and between-day variability of emotions were assessed through adolescent report at 3-month
intervals across a 1 year period. Level and variability of the four emotions contributed to changes in anxiety disorder and
depressive symptoms more consistently than to changes in aggressive behavior. All four emotions were predictive of changes
in internalizing problems, while anger played the most prominent role in the development of aggressive behavior. Variability
of emotions contributed to changes in anxiety disorder symptoms, while heightened levels of negative emotions and diminished
happiness contributed to changes in depression. Results suggested somewhat stronger effects of negative affect on aggressive
behavior for females than for males. Results underscore the role of emotion dysregulation in the development of psychopathology. 相似文献
297.
Otgaar H Smeets T Merckelbach H Jelicic M Verschuere B Galliot AM van Riel L 《Memory & cognition》2011,39(6):1033-1041
Studies have shown that survival processing leads to superior memorability. The aim of the present study was to examine whether
this survival recall advantage might result from stereotype activation. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a pilot study
and two experiments in which participants were primed with stereotypes (Experiment 1, professor and elderly person; Experiment
2, survival-stereotype). In Experiment 1, 120 undergraduates were randomly assigned to a survival, professor stereotype, elderly
person stereotype, or moving scenario and rated words for their relevance to the imagined scenario. In Experiment 2, 75 undergraduates
were given a survival, survival-stereotype (based on our pilot study), or moving scenario. Both experiments showed that survival
processing leads to a greater recall advantage over the stereotype groups and control group. These data indicate that the
mere activation of stereotypes cannot explain the survival recall advantage. 相似文献
298.
Explaining after by before: Basic aspects of a dynamic systems approach to the study of development 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The basic properties of a dynamic systems approach of development are illustrated by contrasting two simple equations. One, yt+1 = f (yt), is characteristic of dynamic systems models. The other, yi = f (xi), refers to what, for the sake of simplicity, is referred to as the standard developmental approach. We give illustrations from cognitive, language and social development to show the characteristic differences of these two types of models and show their complementarity. The article further compares the “Bloomington” with the “Groningen” approach to dynamic systems theorizing in developmental psychology. It continues with a discussion of two important questions. One involves the issue of measurement and the nature of developmental variables from the viewpoint of dynamic systems. The second concerns the question of short- and long-term time scales in developmental models, which is discussed on the basis of an example, namely dyadic interaction of young children in the context of different social statuses. 相似文献
299.
Hierarchical agglomerative cluster analysis (HACA) may yield different solutions under permutations of the input order of the data. This instability is caused by ties, either in the initial proximity matrix or arising during agglomeration. The authors recommend to repeat the analysis on a large number of random permutations of the rows and columns of the proximity matrix and select a solution with the highest goodness-of-fit. This approach was implemented in an SPSS add-in, PermuCLUSTER, which can perform all HACA methods of SPSS. Analyses of 2 data sets show that (a) results are affected by input order, (b) instability in one method co-occurs with instability in other methods, and (c) some instability effects are more dramatic because they occur at higher agglomeration levels. 相似文献
300.
Maria van der Schaar 《Synthese》2011,180(3):391-417
The notion of cognitive act is of importance for an epistemology that is apt for constructive type theory, and for epistemology
in general. Instead of taking knowledge attributions as the primary use of the verb ‘to know’ that needs to be given an account
of, and understanding a first-person knowledge claim as a special case of knowledge attribution, the account of knowledge
that is given here understands first-person knowledge claims as the primary use of the verb ‘to know’. This means that a cognitive
act is an act that counts as cognitive from a first-person point of view. The method of linguistic phenomenology is used to
explain or elucidate our epistemic notions. One of the advantages of the theory is that an answer can be given to some of
the problems in modern epistemology, such as the Gettier problem. 相似文献