全文获取类型
收费全文 | 316篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
318篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 39篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 22篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有318条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
242.
Dianne A. van Hemert Fons J. R. van de Vijver Ype H. Poortinga James Georgas 《Personality and individual differences》2002,33(8)
The question was examined as to whether scores at the individual level and scores at the country level on the four scales of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) have the same psychological meaning. Using data of 24 countries, it was found that the EPQ has different factorial structures at both levels. Both the Lie scale and the Psychoticism scale were shown to jeopardize cross-level equivalence. For further exploration of the meaning of the EPQ scales within countries and between countries country-level correlations were calculated with a variety of country characteristics such as Gross National Product, political indices, religiosity, Hofstede's measures, and subjective well-being. Significant findings for 38 countries included correlations of the EPQ scales with Hofstede's Masculinity, Diener's Subjective Well-Being, religiosity, the number of deaths in a country due to political violence, and bribery. The most striking finding was a substantial negative correlation of the Lie scale with Gross National Product and other wealth-related indices. 相似文献
243.
244.
Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Vohs KD Baumeister RF Schmeichel BJ Twenge JM Nelson NM Tice DM 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2008,94(5):883-898
The current research tested the hypothesis that making many choices impairs subsequent self-control. Drawing from a limited-resource model of self-regulation and executive function, the authors hypothesized that decision making depletes the same resource used for self-control and active responding. In 4 laboratory studies, some participants made choices among consumer goods or college course options, whereas others thought about the same options without making choices. Making choices led to reduced self-control (i.e., less physical stamina, reduced persistence in the face of failure, more procrastination, and less quality and quantity of arithmetic calculations). A field study then found that reduced self-control was predicted by shoppers' self-reported degree of previous active decision making. Further studies suggested that choosing is more depleting than merely deliberating and forming preferences about options and more depleting than implementing choices made by someone else and that anticipating the choice task as enjoyable can reduce the depleting effect for the first choices but not for many choices. 相似文献
245.
Elise D 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2008,56(1):73-98
A developmental narrative is presented that centers on bodily based narcissistic injury and sense of shame in response to unrequited oedipal longings. Through an experience of oedipal defeat in relation to both mother and father, a female sense of inadequacy and shame may be internalized and accepted as one's identity, in contrast to the male phallic-omnipotent trajectory. The demise of genital narcissism in females can underlie various expressions of pervasive inhibition and failure to actualize desire. The thesis offered goes beyond separation-individuation theory in suggesting that girls may inhibit sexuality and aggression, and themselves more generally, due to a representation of self as "not having what it takes" genitally, and then bodily and psychically. Mental representations of the self, based on positive imagery of the female body, are needed to give voice to a woman's bodily experience and sexual desire and agency in various realms. Two clinical vignettes illustrate female inhibitions in sexuality and in professional ambition as understood within the framework presented. 相似文献
246.
Dianne C. Berry 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1991,43(4):881-906
This study looks at whether or not a crucial role is played by the learner's own actions in tasks known to give rise to implicit learning. Experiment 1 shows that experience of watching another person controlling Berry and Broadbent's (1984) sugar production and person interaction tasks has no effect on subsequent control performance. Experiment 2 demonstrates that this lack of effect of observing is limited to tasks where the underlying relationship is not obvious or salient. In Experiment 3 the length of the observation period is doubled, but this still has no beneficial effect on subsequent control performance. Experiments 4 and 5 address the question of what it is about controlling itself that leads to learning. They examine whether it is the decision-making component or the physical interaction component that is important. The results emphasize the importance of the role of action in learning to control these tasks. They show that decision must be tied to action in order to be maximally effective, at least in the early stages of learning. 相似文献
247.
248.
Patient cultural backgrounds strongly influence decision-making processes and outcomes in genetic counseling. The present
study investigated influences of culture and acculturation on prenatal decision making processes of native Palestinians and
Palestinian Americans. Seventeen native Palestinians and 14 first-generation, Palestinian Americans were interviewed and asked
to imagine themselves as patients in hypothetical premarital and prenatal situations. Five major issues were investigated:
1) Influence of family history of an inherited condition on pre-marital decisions; 2) Perceptions of non-directive genetic
counselor statements regarding options; 3) Role of gender in prenatal decisions; 4) Gender differences in emotional expression;
and 5) Role of family and society in prenatal decisions. Several similarities and differences in native Palestinian and Palestinian
American responses were obtained. Similarities appear to be due to common cultural roots, while differences may be due to
acculturation. Practice and research recommendations are provided.
An erratum to this article can be found at 相似文献
249.
Dianne Elise 《Psychoanalytic Dialogues》2013,23(3):284-294
In this discussion I address the multidimensionality embedded within the oedipal phase of development and contend that the elaboration and working through of unrequited oedipal longing within the transference–countertransference matrix is one of the most profound gifts of an analysis. The child’s confrontation with thwarted desire (generationally) for either parent is central to the development of the mind, the personality, and one’s erotic life. I also argue that the experience of oedipal exclusion becomes not solely a response to a generational boundary but to a gender boundary discouraging same-sex love. I speak for retaining a conceptualization of an oedipal complex without losing any of the complexity of individual experience. In describing our oedipal lineages for erotic life as resting upon an interwoven layering of maternal and paternal oedipal desires, my aim is to continue a project to rehabilitate the oedipal concept from heteronormative presumptions and from being (mis)understood in a reductive manner that alienates rather than engages. 相似文献
250.
Michael J. Platow Duncan Mills Dianne Morrison 《European journal of social psychology》2000,30(1):69-81
Based upon a self‐categorisation analysis of social influence (Turner, 1991), we predicted that individuals who self‐categorise with the source of a communication would align their own private attitudes more closely with the source when that source was distributively fair rather than unfair in an intragroup context. We expected this pattern to reverse in an intergroup context when the unfairness was ingroup favouring. These expectations were confirmed in a laboratory experiment (N=101). The data suggest that neither source similarity nor source fairness serve simply as persuasion cues to which individuals thoughtlessly conform. We argue, instead that, once self‐categorised, individuals: (1) actively attend to an ingroup member's behaviours and the context in which they occur, and (2) are influenced only by a source who provides some form of social identity enhancement, either by being fair in an intragroup context (Lind & Tyler, 1988) or ingroup favouring in an intergroup context (Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献