全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1468篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
1495篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 156篇 |
2012年 | 23篇 |
2011年 | 38篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 34篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 65篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 54篇 |
2003年 | 59篇 |
2002年 | 45篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 23篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 20篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 25篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 23篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
1970年 | 12篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1967年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1495条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Lawrence J. Sanna 《Current directions in psychological science》2000,9(5):168-173
Recent advances in theory and research on the relations among mental simulation, affect, and personality are summarized and combined. Research has shown that (a) affect and mental simulations can influence each other, (b) mental simulations can serve diverse self-motives, and (c) personality characteristics are related to divergent functions of mental simulations. Findings in these three areas are synthesized into a conceptual framework on the basis of three attributes of mental simulations: (a) time, whether simulations are prospective or retrospective; (b) direction, whether simulations are upward or downward; and (c) focus, whether simulations are contrasted or assimilated. This integrated framework not only may enable a greater understanding of existing findings, but also suggests novel and unique predictions for future research on understanding personality processes, automaticity in simulations, and coping with life events. 相似文献
72.
Rhoades LJ 《Science and engineering ethics》2000,6(1):95-107
This paper discusses ten lessons learned since 1989 about handling allegations of scientific misconduct involving biomedical
and behavioral research supported by the U. S. Public Health Service.
An earlier version of this paper was presented at a symposium, Scientific Misconduct: An International Perspective, organised by The Medical University of Warsaw, 16 November, 1998.
The opinions expressed herein are those of the author and do not necessarily represent the views of the Office of Research
Integrity, the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, or any other federal agency. 相似文献
73.
Lawrence Greenfield Douglas Fountain 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2000,22(4):353-364
Data for 422 methadone treatment clients in the National Treatment Improvement Evaluation Study (NTIES) were analyzed. Clients maintained continuously in methadone treatment for longer than 12 months and clients who leftbetween 3–12 months were compared with clients treated for less than 3 months. Additionally, clients treated for 3–12 months who had short follow-up periods (6-month average) were compared with 3–12-month clients with long follow-up periods (11-month average). Positive treatment outcomes includinglower drug use, reduced risk of viral infectionand sexually transmitted disease (through needle sharing and multiple sex partners), and less criminality wereassociated with both longer duration treatment and shorter follow-up periods. The findings suggested that continuous methadone treatment of 12 or more months is optimal, whereas stays of less than3 months may be ineffective. Furthermore, stays of 3–12 months are likelyto be beneficial over a relatively short time span, for example 6 months. 相似文献
74.
G. Keith Humphrey Diane Skowbo Lawrence A. Symons Andrew M. Herbert Christina L. Grant 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1994,56(4):405-413
Five experiments reexamined color aftereffects contingent on the semantic properties of text (Allan, Siegel, Collins, & MacQueen, 1989). The influence of different assessment techniques and the effect of eye movements and overlapping contour information on the induction of color aftereffects by word and nonword letter strings were determined. Experiment 1 showed that no aftereffect was found when a traditional method of assessing color aftereffects was used. Experiments 2 and 4 demonstrated color aftereffects forboth words and nonwords, but only when subjects fixated the same locus during induction and testing and only when assessed with the technique described by Allan et al. (1989). If, however, eye movements were made during induction, no color aftereffect was obtained (Experiment 3). Induction to nontext patterns with properties similar to those of text but with fewer overlapping contours resulted in a strong color aftereffect (Experiment 5). These results suggest that the color aftereffect contingent on text is very weak and is not dependent on semantic factors, but that it is a product of induction to local color and orientation information. 相似文献
75.
HIV/AIDS‐related (HAR) stigma is still a prevalent problem in Sub‐Saharan Africa, and has been found to be related to mental health of HIV‐positive individuals. However, no studies in the Sub‐Saharan African context have yet examined the relationship between HAR stigma and mental health among HIV‐negative, HIV‐affected adults and families; nor have any studies in this context yet examined stigma as an ecological construct predicting mental health outcomes through supra‐individual (setting level) and individual levels of influence. Multilevel modeling was used to examine multilevel, ecological relationships between HAR stigma and mental health among child and caregiver pairs from a systematic, community‐representative sample of 508 HIV‐affected households nested within 24 communities in KwaZulu‐Natal, South Africa. Two distinct dimensions of HAR stigma were measured: individual stigmatizing attitudes, and perceptions of community normative stigma. Findings suggest that individual‐level HAR stigma significantly predicts individual mental health (depression and anxiety) among HIV‐affected adults; and that community‐level HAR stigma significantly predicts both individual‐level mental health outcomes (anxiety) among HIV‐affected adults, and mental health outcomes (PTSD and externalizing behavior scores) among HIV‐affected children. Differentiated patterns of relationships were found using the two different stigma measures. These findings of unique relationships identified when utilizing two conceptually distinct stigma measures, at two levels of analysis (individual and community) suggest that HAR stigma in this context should be conceptualized as a multilevel, multidimensional construct. These findings have important implications both for mental health interventions and for interventions to reduce HAR stigma in this context. 相似文献
76.
Rumination has long been considered a verbal thought process, though emerging evidence suggests that some individuals dwell on maladaptive imagery. This series of studies evaluated imagery and verbal thought during experimentally induced rumination and distraction. In Study 1, imagery and verbal thought during rumination resulted in similar increases in negative affect. Greater imagery during distraction, on the other hand, was associated with greater decreases in negative affect while verbal thought was not related to affect change. Given that greater verbal thought was reported in the rumination condition and greater imagery was reported in the distraction condition, Study 2 evaluated whether the rumination/distraction induction was confounded by concurrent induction of imagery or verbal thought. The rumination prompts induced both rumination and verbal thought and the distraction prompts induced both distraction and imagery. Using a revised induction, Study 3 tested whether imagery and verbal thought during rumination and distraction impacted affective response. Rumination maintained negative affect and distraction relieved negative affect, regardless of the degree to which imagery or verbal thought was experienced. This paper provides evidence that imagery-based rumination is just as impairing as verbally-based rumination and highlights imagery-based distraction as a potentially effective alternative to rumination. 相似文献
77.
Michael N. Dalili Lawrence Schofield-Toloza Marcus R. Munafò Ian S. Penton-Voak 《Cognition & emotion》2017,31(5):858-867
Many cognitive bias modification (CBM) tasks use facial expressions of emotion as stimuli. Some tasks use unique facial stimuli, while others use composite stimuli, given evidence that emotion is encoded prototypically. However, CBM using composite stimuli may be identity- or emotion-specific, and may not generalise to other stimuli. We investigated the generalisability of effects using composite faces in two experiments. Healthy adults in each study were randomised to one of four training conditions: two stimulus-congruent conditions, where same faces were used during all phases of the task, and two stimulus-incongruent conditions, where faces of the opposite sex (Experiment 1) or faces depicting another emotion (Experiment 2) were used after the modification phase. Our results suggested that training effects generalised across identities. However, our results indicated only partial generalisation across emotions. These findings suggest effects obtained using composite stimuli may extend beyond the stimuli used in the task but remain emotion-specific. 相似文献
78.
Chiang Chien-pei Yang Pao-chien C. Gilbert Wrenn Lawrence M. Brammer 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1966,45(2):130-133
The Republic of China is developing its guidance programs energetically and rapidly. The Chinese Guidance Association, a young but vigorous professional association, is devoted to research and development of China's student and industrial personnel programs. This APGA sister association has taken long strides ahead in the past eight years through organization of experimental school and college guidance programs, counselor education, development of tests, and distribution of vocational information materials. It has an ambitious and promising “five-year development plan” blocked out for the period 1966–71. 相似文献
79.
Emerson James G. Young Richard K. Thornton Edward E. Lacour Lawrence 《Pastoral Psychology》1966,17(1):59-66
Pastoral Psychology - 相似文献
80.
Rosenthal SL Griffith JO Succop PA Biro FM Lewis LM DeVellis RF Stanberry LR 《Adolescence》2002,37(145):83-92
This longitudinal study examined adolescent girls' perception of control over sexually transmitted disease (STD) acquisition. Participants were asked questions regarding their perception of their STD locus of control (internal control; control by parents, partners, peers, and health care providers; and chance) at two waves of data collection. Of the 116 participants (mean age = 17 years), 82% were African-American and 18% were Caucasian. Responses to the measure of locus of control were significantly correlated across a 6-month interval. The responses regarding internal control, control by partner, and chance were not related to the acquisition of an STD in the next 6 months. Further, they were not influenced by an STD in the preceding 6 months. These results indicate that responses to a locus of control measure were stable over a 6-month period, and internal, partner, and chance locus of control perceptions seem to be neither determined by STD experience nor directly related to future STD acquisition. However, understanding an individual's locus of control may be helpful in providing appropriate counseling. Future research could examine how adolescent girls form their perceptions of control over STD acquisition. 相似文献