首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7571篇
  免费   304篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2021年   61篇
  2020年   116篇
  2019年   129篇
  2018年   165篇
  2017年   189篇
  2016年   187篇
  2015年   165篇
  2014年   190篇
  2013年   923篇
  2012年   302篇
  2011年   309篇
  2010年   191篇
  2009年   224篇
  2008年   316篇
  2007年   312篇
  2006年   280篇
  2005年   278篇
  2004年   270篇
  2003年   243篇
  2002年   248篇
  2001年   113篇
  2000年   109篇
  1999年   120篇
  1998年   126篇
  1997年   86篇
  1996年   86篇
  1995年   97篇
  1994年   84篇
  1993年   71篇
  1992年   76篇
  1991年   74篇
  1990年   62篇
  1989年   68篇
  1988年   66篇
  1987年   59篇
  1986年   61篇
  1985年   77篇
  1984年   75篇
  1983年   76篇
  1982年   64篇
  1981年   68篇
  1980年   65篇
  1979年   45篇
  1978年   67篇
  1977年   58篇
  1976年   68篇
  1975年   66篇
  1974年   59篇
  1973年   44篇
  1972年   38篇
排序方式: 共有7877条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
981.
982.
The authors sought to examine whether rumination about psychologically painful, though nontraumatic, interpersonal transgressions is associated with increased salivary cortisol. They measured salivary cortisol, rumination about a transgression, fear and anger regarding the transgressor, perceived painfulness of the transgression, and positive and negative mood in 115 undergraduates who had experienced an interpersonal transgression during the previous 7 days. They obtained measurements on as many as 5 occasions separated by approximately 14 days each. On occasions when participants reported that they had been ruminating to a degree that was greater than was typical for them, they had higher levels of salivary cortisol than was typical for them. The rumination- cortisol association appeared to be mediated by fear of the transgressor. Rumination about even moderately painful but nontraumatic life events and associated emotions are related to biological changes that may subserve social goals such as avoiding social threats. Items from the rumination scale are appended.  相似文献   
983.
984.
985.
986.
Paul Lorenzen 《Synthese》1956,10(1):181-186
Ohne Zusammenfassung  相似文献   
987.
How do people come up with humorous ideas? In creative cognition research, exposure to good examples sometimes causes fixation (people get “stuck” on the examples) but other times sparks inspiration (people's responses are more creative). The present research examined the effects of funny and unfunny examples on joke production. A sample of 175 adults read scenarios that they completed with funny responses. All participants were instructed to be funny, but before responding they read (a) funny responses as examples of good responses to emulate, (b) unfunny responses as examples of poor responses to avoid, or (c) no examples. The participants’ own responses were rated for funniness and for similarity to the example responses, and response times were recorded. Reading either funny or unfunny examples, compared to no examples, caused people to come up with funnier jokes. Similarity to the examples was low in all conditions, so fixation was relatively modest, but people who saw unfunny examples spent more time coming up with their responses. Taken together, the findings support the growing literature showing that examples are often inspiring rather than constraining, and they imply that good and bad examples spark creative thought via different paths.  相似文献   
988.
The study investigated perceived religious discrimination and three facets of Muslim identity (psychological, behavioural, and visible) as predictors of psychological well‐being (life satisfaction and psychological symptoms) of 153 Muslim women in New Zealand. The results indicated that although visibility (wearing hijab) was associated with greater perceived discrimination, it predicted positive psychological outcomes. Analysis further revealed that the psychological (pride, belongingness, and centrality) and behavioural (engaging in Islamic practices) facets of Muslim identity moderated the relationship between perceived religious discrimination and well‐being. A strong psychological affiliation with Islam exacerbated the negative relationship between perceived religious discrimination and well‐being. Conversely, engaging in Islamic practices buffered the negative impact of discrimination. The research highlights the complexity of Muslim identity in diasporic women.  相似文献   
989.
990.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号