全文获取类型
收费全文 | 679篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
691篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 75篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 21篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 21篇 |
2008年 | 28篇 |
2007年 | 31篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 17篇 |
2002年 | 20篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有691条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
551.
A modification of the WGTA is described. It retains those features of the original apparatus which make it efficient. In addition, it provides (1) precise and largely automatic control of trial pacing and stimulus presentation, (2) simultaneous acquisition of data on problem-solving and locomotor activity of the S, (3) rapid change to any one of a wide variety of behavioral situations, and (4) thorough but quick cleaning and disinfection of the monkey cage. 相似文献
552.
In order to compare the recognition of objective (defined in three dimensions) and projective (on the frontal-parallel plane) shapes, subjects were asked to identify angles (either objective or projective) on random polygons that were displayed in complex and meaningful photographic slides in the frontal-parallel plane. The subjects’ judgments corresponded much more closely to the objective than to the projective shapes, almost independently of which of the two shapes they were asked to judge and of whether the slides were presented in a normal upright orientation or were rotated 180 deg. The results are incompatible with the strong form of the shape-slant hypothesis, which assumes that the primary perceptual information concerns the properties of the projective retinal image. The results indicate, instead, that a tacit conception of three-dimensional space is primary in the perceptual process. 相似文献
553.
Vanessa Harrar Rebecca Winter Laurence R. Harris 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2008,70(5):807-817
This article compares the properties of apparent motion between a light and a touch with apparent motion between either two lights or two touches. Visual and tactile stimulators were attached to the tips of the two index fingers that were held apart at different distances. Subjects rated the quality of apparent motion between each stimulus combination for a range of stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). Subjects reported perceiving apparent motion between all three stimulus combinations. For light—light visual apparent motion, the preferred SOA and the direction threshold SOAs increased as the distance between the stimuli increased (consistent with Korte’s third law of apparent motion). Touch—touch apparent motion also obeyed Korte’s third law, but over a smaller range of distances, showing that proprioceptive information concerning the position of the fingers is integrated into the tactile motion system. The threshold and preferred SOAs for visuotactile apparent motion did not vary with distance, suggesting a different mechanism for multimodal apparent motion. 相似文献
554.
In 1995, the Pedigree Standardization Task Force (PSTF) of the National Society of Genetic Counselors (NSGC) proposed a system
of pedigree nomenclature. Recently, the PSTF (now called the Pedigree Standardization Work Group or PSWG) sought evidence
that the published symbols met the needs of health professionals, were incorporated into health professional training and
were utilized in publications. We searched PubMed and reference lists of select publications, reviewed the Instructions for
Authors of several journals, searched the websites of professional societies, sought comment from the membership of the NSGC,
and looked at recommendations and training practices of various health professional organizations. Many journals still do
not cite specific standards for pedigrees, but those found cited the PSTF nomenclature. We did not find significant objections
or alternatives to the 1995 nomenclature. Based on our review, we propose only a few minor stylistic changes to the pedigree
symbols. The pedigree nomenclature of the NSGC is the only consistently acknowledged standard for drawing a family health
history. We recommend regular and continued review of these pedigree standards to determine if additional symbols are needed
to accommodate changes in clinical practice to ensure that the symbols continue to meet the needs of health professionals
and researchers as well as adhere to evolving ethical and privacy standards. All health professionals, trainees, and researchers
should be made aware of the utility of using a common pedigree nomenclature in clinical practice and publication. This will
become particularly important as electronic medical records become more widely utilized. 相似文献
555.
Philosophy textbooks typically treat bioethics as a form of "applied ethics"-i.e., an attempt to apply a moral theory, like utilitarianism, to controversial ethical issues in biology and medicine. Historians, however, can find virtually no cases in which applied philosophical moral theory influenced ethical practice in biology or medicine. In light of the absence of historical evidence, the authors of this paper advance an alternative model of the historical relationship between philosophical ethics and medical ethics, the appropriation model. They offer two historical case studies to illustrate the ways in which physicians have "appropriated" concepts and theory fragments from philosophers, and demonstrate how appropriated moral philosophy profoundly influenced the way medical morality was conceived and practiced. 相似文献
556.
557.
558.
559.
560.
This article presents a response to the comment by B. J. Bushman, D. Romer, and P. E. Jamieson (2015). This reply addresses 2 issues raised by the commenters. First, they claim they and others have not made sensationalistic statements linking violent media to horrific acts of real‐world violence. In response, we supply numerous examples of sensationalistic statements made by them and others. Second, they claim they did not expect violence in films to be related to violent behavior among adults, but only among youths. However, by examining homicide arrests and homicide gun mortality rates among youths, we found that as films have become increasingly violent over time, both homicide arrests and gun‐related homicides have tended to decrease among this age group. 相似文献