全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53516篇 |
免费 | 642篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
54164篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 408篇 |
2019年 | 529篇 |
2018年 | 3957篇 |
2017年 | 3292篇 |
2016年 | 2868篇 |
2015年 | 687篇 |
2014年 | 687篇 |
2013年 | 2752篇 |
2012年 | 1679篇 |
2011年 | 3353篇 |
2010年 | 3076篇 |
2009年 | 2151篇 |
2008年 | 2537篇 |
2007年 | 3014篇 |
2006年 | 867篇 |
2005年 | 934篇 |
2004年 | 887篇 |
2003年 | 755篇 |
2002年 | 728篇 |
2001年 | 1098篇 |
2000年 | 1129篇 |
1999年 | 809篇 |
1998年 | 370篇 |
1997年 | 325篇 |
1995年 | 328篇 |
1992年 | 642篇 |
1991年 | 596篇 |
1990年 | 580篇 |
1989年 | 550篇 |
1988年 | 546篇 |
1987年 | 484篇 |
1986年 | 486篇 |
1985年 | 534篇 |
1984年 | 436篇 |
1983年 | 401篇 |
1982年 | 317篇 |
1979年 | 478篇 |
1978年 | 333篇 |
1977年 | 312篇 |
1976年 | 323篇 |
1975年 | 365篇 |
1974年 | 435篇 |
1973年 | 468篇 |
1972年 | 403篇 |
1971年 | 344篇 |
1970年 | 322篇 |
1969年 | 338篇 |
1968年 | 420篇 |
1967年 | 371篇 |
1966年 | 361篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
811.
Validation of the Differential Emotions Scale in 613 mothers 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Mothers (N=613) with infants who took the Differential Emotions Scale (DES) were compared in a cross-validation study by factor analysis to 957 high school and college students. Despite differences in factor-analytic technique, similarities in factor structure suggested that the DES discriminates the majority of fundamental emotions as posited by Izard.Robert Emde is supported by Research Scientists Award No. 5 KO5 MH 36808 and NIMH Project Grant No. 2 RO1 MH 22803. The authors wish to acknowledge the critical support of the Developmental Psychobiology Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Medical School, and wish to express their gratitude to Carroll Izard for allowing us to publish his factor-analytic results. 相似文献
812.
The present study expands the typical Barnum effect paradigm by investigating reactions to feedback in a group setting. People initially participated in a group experience (8 per group), and then were given bogus positive or negative feedback (the favorability manipulation) purportedly prepared by either the group leader or another group member (the source status manipulation). The answerability manipulation led participants to believe that they either would or would not have to share their reactions to the feedback with the person who prepared it. Positive feedback was rated as more accurate and accepted more highly than the negative feedback, though no differences in recall of either the positive or negative feedback emerged. Additionally, a pattern of results revealed that the feedback from the group leader generated greater perceived accuracy, acceptance, and recall than did feedback from another group member. Implications for the impact of feedback given by a high status person in group settings are discussed. 相似文献
813.
The resistance of the Personal Orientation Dimensions (POD) to deliberate attempts at creating a favorable impression was investigated. Two groups of college students were used: (a) 21 students completed the POD under standard instructions, then received information about actualization, and completed the POD again under instructions to Create a very good impression; (b) 21 students completed the POD under standard instructions, then received information not related to actualization, and completed the POD again under instructions to create a very good impression. An analyses indicated that providing students with information about actualization did not facilitate their ability to create a favorable impression on the POD. The POD profiles produced by both groups changed significantly, and in a similar direction from pretest to posttest. Evidence is presented suggesting the existence of a "lie profile" which distinguishes deliberate attempts to create a favorable impression on the POD. These findings provide support for the resistance of the POD to deliberate attempts at creating a favorable impression. 相似文献
814.
A hierarchical simple structure factor solution was obtained for item scores from a psychometrized battery of Piagetian-type tasks individually administered to 660 kindergarten children. At the first level of factors were the Piagetian theoretical entities of seriation, classification, conservation of number, and temporal and spatial reasoning, with numeration loading on seriation and classification as indicated by the theory. At the second level were two factors identified as Piaget's logicomathematical and infralogical operations. A third-level factor was identified as a g factor. The association of the Piagetian-type tasks with kindergarden and first grade academic achievement was through the third-level g factor. The second-level factors appeared to be similar to Vernon's k:m and v:ed but, like the first level factors, made no important contribution to predicted achievement above that contributed by the g factor. A socioeconomic factor made no unique contribution to predicted first grade achievement. It was concluded that the Piagetian concrete operations variables have no unique role in the prediction of kindergarten or first grade academic achievement. 相似文献
815.
Boris B. Baltes Cara C. Bauer Linda M. Bajdo Christopher P. Parker 《Journal of business and psychology》2002,17(1):3-17
Previous monomethod research has found mixed support for nonlinear effects between certain job characteristics (e.g., job autonomy, job complexity) and outcome variables (e.g., job performance). We hypothesized that these weak nonlinear findings may be due to the prevalence of monomethod research that can lead to a lack of complete measurement and/or introduce common methods variance, either of which may mask the true shape of relationships. Using hierarchical regression analyses and a multitrait–multimethod (MTMM) research design, we found strikingly different results between monomethod and MTMM data when considering the relationships between three psychological climate variables and job satisfaction. While the monomethod results mirrored earlier inconclusive findings, the MTMM data indicated that nonlinear equations explained significantly more of the relationship between all three climate dimensions and job satisfaction. These results suggest that the use of MTMM data allows one to more effectively test for nonlinear effects. Furthermore, these nonlinear results suggest that the format of employee questionnaires should probably change from asking how much an employee has of certain constructs to asking does the employee want more or less of these constructs. 相似文献
816.
The miracle argument for scientific realism can be cast in two forms: according to the miraculous theory argument, realism is the only position which does not make the empirical successes of particular theories miraculous. According to the miraculous choice argument, realism is the only position which does not render the fact that empirically successful theories have been chosen a miracle. A vast literature discusses the miraculous theory argument, but the miraculous choice argument has been unjustifiably neglected. I raise two objections to Richard Boyd's defense of the latter: (1) we have no miracle free account of the emergence of take-off theories and (2) the anti-realist can account for the non-miraculous choice of empirically successful theories by attributing mere empirical adequacy to background theory. I argue that the availability of extra-empirical criteria that are arguably truth conductive but not theory-laden suffices to answer (1), and the unavailability of extra-empirical criteria that are conductive to empirical adequacy but not necessarily to truth (and are also not theory-laden) constitutes to reply to (2). The prospects for a realist victory are at least somewhat promising, on a controversial assumption about the rate at which empirically successful theories emerge. 相似文献
817.
818.
819.
820.
Warren R. Stanton 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2002,21(2):176-198
Stage theories are commonly used to represent the structure of cognitive processes in the domains of cognitive processing
and cognitive development. Hierarchical unidimensional theories consisting of a single sequence of stages through which development
proceeds in one direction only have predominated. However, a review of stage theories revealed that a two-dimensional, interactive
(multidimensional) structure is warranted. Particularly in the area of cognitive development, results of research have supported
subdivision of the stages of prominent unidimensional theories. Following this trend a small number of theorists have developed
multidimensional theories, most of which consist of a sequence of stages in which each stage contains a number of subsidiary
stages or levels. The emergence of a similar trend in other domains is examined, and the prospects of using an existing multidimensional
framework to enable future comparisons and integration of different stage theories is explored. 相似文献