全文获取类型
收费全文 | 345篇 |
免费 | 24篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
371篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 48篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有371条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Strother L Kubovy M 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2006,32(2):226-234
The authors conducted 3 experiments to explore the roles of curvature, density, and relative proximity in the perceptual organization of ambiguous dot patterns. To this end, they developed a new family of regular dot patterns that tend to be perceptually grouped into parallel contours, dot-sampled structured grids (DSGs). DSGs are similar to the dot lattices used to study grouping by proximity, except that only one of the potential organizations is rectilinear; the others are curvilinear. The authors used the method of M. Kubovy and J. Wagemans (1995) to study grouping by proximity in DSGs. They found that in the competition between the most likely organizations, one rectilinear and the other curvilinear, the latter is more salient. This phenomenon cannot be explained by contemporary accounts of grouping by proximity or contour integration. 相似文献
92.
93.
Carlsson AH Bjorvatn C Engebretsen LF Berglund G Natvig GK 《Journal of genetic counseling》2004,13(5):425-445
The aim of this multicenter study was to explore associations between psychosocial factors (general self-efficacy, perceived availability of social support, cancer-related distress) and health-related quality of life, among individuals at risk for hereditary cancer. One-hundred and twenty one participants with a family history of breast-cancer or colorectal cancer answered a questionnaire 2-4 weeks prior to genetic counseling. The two dimensions of the health-related quality of life measure, mental and physical health were both used as outcome variables. Multiple regression (linear) analyses revealed that increasing degrees of cancer-related distress was related to decreasing degrees of mental health whereas increasing degrees of self-efficacy and social support were related to increasing degrees of this outcome variable. Self-efficacy, self-esteem support and tangible aid seemed to moderate the relationship between cancer-related distress and mental health. These results suggest that self-efficacy and certain resources of social support buffer the negative association between cancer-related distress and mental health, and might be suitable for interventional efforts. Implications for genetic counseling practice are discussed. 相似文献
94.
Research suggests that the striatum plays an important role in implicit learning (IL). The striatum exhibits marked age-related
morphological and neurochemical losses. Yet, behavioral studies suggest that IL is generally well preserved in old age, and
that age-related differences emerge only when highly complex IL tasks are used. In this review, we integrate behavioral and
neuroimaging evidence on IL in aging. We suggest that relative stability of IL in old age may reflect neural reorganization
that compensates for age-related losses in striatal functions. Specifically, there may be an age-related increase in reliance
on extrastriatal regions (e.g., medial-temporal, frontal) during IL. This reorganization of function may be beneficial under
less taxing performance conditions, but not when task demands become more challenging. 相似文献
95.
An instrument measuring attitudes toward suicide was developed through two postal questionnaire surveys, 1986 and 1996, approaching general population samples of 700 and 1,000 persons, respectively, and with response rates of 75% and 64%. A 10-factor model was obtained and comparisons between waves revealed an overall more permissive and pro-preventional attitude in 1996. The results implied complex relationships at the aggregated level between attitudes and suicidal behavior, depending on type of suicidal behavior, attitude factor, and subgroup. At the individual level, persons answering affirmative on own suicidal behavior, as compared to the nonsuicidal group, held a more permissive and understanding attitude toward suicide. 相似文献
96.
Sex differences exist for several cognitive tasks and estrogen has been suggested to influence these differences. Eighteen men and 18 women were matched on age and estradiol level. Potential sex differences were assessed in episodic memory, semantic memory, verbal fluency, problem solving, and visuospatial ability. Significant sex differences, favoring women, were found for tasks assessing episodic memory. Correlations between estradiol level and cognitive performance were significant for face recognition in females. Since sex differences remained in verbal episodic memory tasks and face recognition despite matched levels of estradiol, circulating estradiol does not appear to be of paramount consequence for observed sex differences in episodic memory. 相似文献
97.
W. Brad Johnson Stevan Lars Nielsen 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》1998,16(2):101-123
Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT), when practiced effectively and flexibly, can be an excellent treatment modality for religious clients. Most of the American population acknowledges some religious belief and/or practice and religious clients sometimes present with unique disturbances and concerns about psychotherapy. In this article we discuss the rapidly changing literature relative to religiousness and mental health and highlight the ethical-professional risks of failing to carefully assess and responsibly manage client religiousness when it is personally and clinically salient. We conclude by outlining a model for focused assessment of religiousness early in REBT and recommend that REBT practitioners consider different components of religiousness and the manner in which they may impact response to treatment. 相似文献
98.
Cognitive neuroscience of ownership and agency 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
99.
Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans 下载免费PDF全文
Schwabe L Oitzl MS Philippsen C Richter S Bohringer A Wippich W Schachinger H 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2007,14(1):109-116
Animal studies provided evidence that stress modulates multiple memory systems, favoring caudate nucleus-based "habit" memory over hippocampus-based "cognitive" memory. However, effects of stress on learning strategy and memory consolidation were not differentiated. We specifically address the effects of psychosocial stress on the applied learning strategy in humans. We designed a spatial learning task that allowed differentiating spatial from stimulus-response learning strategies during acquisition. In 13 subsequent trials, participants (88 male and female students) had to locate a "win" card out of four placed at a fixed location in a 3D model of a room. Relocating one cue in the last trial allowed inferring the applied learning strategy. Half of them participated first in the "Trier Social Stress Test." Salivary cortisol and heart rate measurements were taken. Stressed participants used a stimulus-response strategy significantly more often than controls. Subsequent verbal report revealed that spatial learners had a more complete awareness of response options than stimulus-response learners. Importantly, learning performance was not affected by stress. Taken together, stress prior to learning facilitated simple stimulus-response learning strategies in humans-at the expense of a more cognitive learning strategy. Depending on the context, we consider this as an adaptive response. 相似文献
100.
Event-related functional MRI (fMRI) was used to assess brain activity during encoding of fearful and neutral faces in 12 women and 12 men. In a subsequent memory analysis, the authors separated successful from unsuccessful encoding of both types of faces, based on whether they were remembered or forgotten in a later recognition memory test. Overall, women and men recruited overlapping neural circuitries. Both sexes activated right-sided medial-temporal regions during successful encoding of fearful faces. Successful encoding of neutral faces was associated with left-sided lateral prefrontal and right-sided superior frontal activation in both sexes. In women, relatively greater encoding related activity for neutral faces was seen in the superior parietal and parahippocampal cortices. By contrast, men activated the left and right superior/middle frontal cortex more than women during successful encoding of the same neutral faces. These findings suggest that women and men use similar neural networks to encode facial information, with only subtle sex differences observed for neutral faces. 相似文献