全文获取类型
收费全文 | 345篇 |
免费 | 24篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
371篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 48篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有371条查询结果,搜索用时 109 毫秒
11.
Michael Rönnlund Lars Nyberg Lars Bäckman Lars-Göran Nilsson 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(3):182-201
Motor activity during encoding of verbal information has been suggested to reduce age differences in episodic memory. Here we examined memory for sentences encoded with enactment (SPTs, subject-performed tasks) or without enactment (VTs, verbal tasks) in a population-based sample consisting of 10 groups ranging in age from 35 to 80 years (N = 1000). Memory performance was assessed by immediate free- and category-cued recall. Degree of clustering was measured by the adjusted ratio of clustering score. Recall of cognitive activities served as a complementary measure of memory for performed tasks. Sentence recall showed a gradual decline across age, of about the same magnitude for SPTs and VTs, in both free and cued recall. Clustering in free recall was higher for SPTs than for VTs, but there were no age differences in clustering. A pattern of gradual decline from age 35 was observed also in activity recall, regardless of whether the activities involved motor activity or not. Across the memory measures, differences in education accounted for all of the age-related variance in performance among the younger (35-55 years) but not the older groups (60-80 years), suggesting that genuine aging effects in these measures are more prominent in old age. Together, the results indicate that age differences in episodic memory, in line with most, if not all, types of encoding support, generalize across the performed/non-performed distinction. 相似文献
12.
Reza Habib Lars Nyberg 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(3):257-273
ABSTRACT Studies of successful aging have typically defined elderly who fall in the upper end of a distribution of test scores as successful. A different definition of successful aging requires that older adults fall at or above the mean level of younger adults and maintain this level over time. Here we examined this definition of successful aging in a sample of 1463 individuals between the ages of 50 of 85. Based on principal coordinate analysis of cognitive and non-cognitive variables, we identified a group of 55 (8.3%) 70–85 years olds that were high functioning. This group of elderly showed elevated performance on a range of cognitive tasks. Non-cognitive factors that characterized this group included education and subjective health. The participants were retested 5 years later and the same type of analysis was repeated. Of the remaining individuals who initially were classified as high functioning, 18 (35%) remained high functioning and thus met the definition for successful aging. Years of education was a significant predictor of who remained successful over time. 相似文献
13.
14.
15.
The role of task persistence in young adolescence for successful educational and occupational attainment in middle adulthood 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The main purpose of the present study was to investigate the importance of task persistence in young adolescence for successful educational and occupational attainment in middle adulthood. Data from age 13 (N = 1,092) and adult age (age 43 for women, N = 569 and age 47 for men, N = 393) were taken from the Swedish longitudinal research program entitled "Individual Development and Adaptation." In line with previous research, task persistence was found to be related to changes in grades between age 13 and age 16, over and above other childhood factors. Task persistence at age 13 was also a significant predictor of both income and occupational level in middle adulthood for the men, controlling for a number of childhood factors (including intelligence), and even when educational attainment in adulthood was taken into account. Finally, task persistence was related to educational attainment in adulthood. The authors suggest that task persistence is a second fundamental factor besides general mental ability, influencing attainment within the area of working life and education. The influence of task persistence is discussed in form of personality-environment selection mechanisms. 相似文献
16.
Bumblebees (Bombus spp.) foraging in the field typically reject flowers where they detect the olfactory footprints of previous visitors and hence
avoid recently emptied inflorescences. A growing number of studies have begun to illustrate that associative learning shapes
the development of this process, in both bumblebees and other bee species. This raises the question of what the default response
to such marks is, but little is known about how inexperienced foragers use social information. Here, we offered flower-naive
bees a choice between scent-marked flowers and unmarked alternatives and found that individuals neither avoided nor preferred
marked flowers. Our findings provide no support for ‘hard-wired’ responses to scent marks in bumblebees and highlight the
importance of associative learning in shaping social information use to match local circumstances. 相似文献
17.
Lars Nyberg Ulrich Olofsson John M. Gardiner Lars-Göran Nilsson 《Psychological research》1997,59(4):231-239
The purpose of this study was to assess retrieval strategy in incidental, intentional, and inclusion tests with word-fragment cues following a levels-of-processing manipulation at study. The results of Exp. 1 showed small levels-of-processing effects in incidental tests, and most subjects reported involuntary rather than voluntary retrieval of study-list words. In an intentional test, although levels of processing had a much greater effect, quite a few subjects also reported involuntary rather than voluntary retrieval of study-list words, and these subjects showed a smaller effect of levels of processing than subjects reporting voluntary retrieval. These results suggest that subjects given instructions for both voluntary and involuntary retrieval of study-list words in an inclusion test might not in fact attempt voluntary retrieval at all, but simply adopt an involuntary retrieval strategy. The results of Exp. 2 provided evidence to support this suggestion. The general implication is that where test contamination refers to subjects' failure to use retrieval strategies in accordance with test instructions, inclusion tests can be contaminated, as well as incidental or intentional tests, and that it is always necessary to obtain converging evidence about the actual strategies subjects use. 相似文献
18.
Lars Nyberg 《Memory (Hove, England)》2013,21(5-6):585-597
Functional neuroimaging studies are beginning to identify neuroanatomical correlates of various cognitive functions. This paper presents results relevant to several theories and phenomena of episodic memory, including component processes of episodic retrieval, encoding specificity, inhibition, item versus source memory, encoding-retrieval overlap, and the picture-superiority effect. Overall, by revealing specific activation patterns, the results provide support for existing theoretical views and they add some unique information which may be important to consider in future attempts to develop cognitive theories of episodic memory. 相似文献
19.
Lars O. White Jia Wu Jessica L. Borelli Linda C. Mayes Michael J. Crowley 《Developmental science》2013,16(6):850-863
Reunion behavior following stressful separations from caregivers is often considered the single most sensitive clue to infant attachment patterns. Extending these ideas to middle childhood/early adolescence, we examined participants' neural responses to reunion with peers who had previously excluded them. We recorded event‐related potentials among nineteen 11‐ to 15‐year‐old youth previously classified on attachment interviews (11 secure and 8 insecure‐dismissing) while they played a virtual ball‐toss game (Cyberball) with peers that involved fair play, exclusion and reunion phases. Compared to secure participants, dismissing participants displayed a greater increment in the N2 during reunion relative to fair play, a neural marker commonly linked to expectancy violation. These data suggest a greater tendency toward continued expectations of rejection among dismissing children, even after cessation of social exclusion. In turn, the link between self‐reported ostracism distress and neural signs of negative expectancy at reunion was moderated by attachment, such that self‐reports were discordant with the neural index of expectancy violation for dismissing, but not for secure children. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=qzoRel5c-4s . 相似文献
20.