全文获取类型
收费全文 | 376篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
386篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 50篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有386条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
Marcel Badra Lars Schulze Eni S. Becker Janna Nonja Vrijsen Babette Renneberg 《Cognition & emotion》2017,31(6):1234-1242
Cognitive models propose that both, negative interpretations of ambiguous social situations and ruminative thoughts about social events contribute to the maintenance of social anxiety disorder. It has further been postulated that ruminative thoughts fuel biased negative interpretations, however, evidence is rare. The present study used a multi-method approach to assess ruminative processing following a social interaction (post-event processing by self-report questionnaire and social rumination by experience sampling method) and negative interpretation bias (via two separate tasks) in a student sample (n?=?51) screened for high (HSA) and low social anxiety (LSA). Results support the hypothesis that group differences in negative interpretations of ambiguous social situations in HSAs vs. LSAs are mediated by higher levels of post-event processing assessed in the questionnaire. Exploratory analyses highlight the potential role of comorbid depressive symptoms. The current findings help to advance the understanding of the association between two cognitive processes involved in social anxiety and stress the importance of ruminative post-event processing. 相似文献
142.
Voluntary actions and their distal effects are intimately related in conscious awareness. When an expected effect follows a voluntary action, the experience of the interval between these events is compressed in time, a phenomenon known as ‘intentional binding’ (IB). Current accounts of IB suggest that it serves to reinforce associations between our goals and our intention to attain these goals via action, and that IB only occurs for self-generated (i.e., intentional) actions. We used a novel approach to study IB in the context of shared intentions and actions. Pairs of participants judged the time of occurrence of actions and events attributed either to oneself or to another agent. We found that IB and subjective agency are not mutually predictive when an action can be attributed to only one of two ‘co-intending’ agents. Our results pose a complication for the prevailing view that IB and subjective agency reflect a common mechanism. 相似文献
143.
144.
We examined the use of lexeme meaning during the processing of spatially unified bilexemic compound words by manipulating both the location and the word frequency of the lexeme that primarily defined the meaning of a compound (i.e., the dominant lexeme). The semantically dominant and nondominant lexemes occupied either the beginning or the ending compound word location, and the beginning and ending lexemes could be either high- or low-frequency words. Three tasks were used--lexical decision, naming, and sentence reading--all of which focused on the effects of lexeme frequency as a function of lexeme dominance. The results revealed a larger word frequency effect for the dominant lexeme in all three tasks. Eye movements during sentence reading further revealed larger word frequency effects for the dominant lexeme via several oculomotor motor measures, including the duration of the first fixation on a compound word. These findings favor theoretical conceptions in which the use of lexeme meaning is an integral part of the compound recognition process. 相似文献
145.
Brogaard's non-indexical version of moral contextualism has two related problems. It is unable to account for the function of truth-governed assertoric moral discourse, since it leaves two (semantically clearheaded) disputants without any incentive to resolve seemingly contradictory moral claims. The moral contextualist could explain why people do feel such an incentive by ascribing false beliefs about the semantic workings of their own language. But, secondly, this leaves Brogaard's moral contextualism looking weaker than a Mackie-style invariantist error theory about morals. The latter is equally non-objectivist, but less revisionist, since it takes the semantics of moral discourse at face value, and can also explain all of Brogaard's other linguistic evidence. 相似文献
146.
Typical patterns of general subjective well-being (SWB) were searched for in a representative longitudinal sample of Swedish
women (N = 272) at age 43 and 49. Cluster analysis at each age separately resulted in a six-cluster solution at both ages. The two
solutions were similar, indicating structural stability across 6 years. Five of the six clusters also showed significant individual
stability. Among these clusters, a generalized positive typical pattern and two generalized negative typical patterns were
found, one characterized by very high negative affect and one characterized by very low global life satisfaction. A cluster
characterized by above average positive and negative affect was also found as well as one characterized by low positive affect.
A strong relationship was found between membership in an extreme cluster and the values in certain SWB-related variables,
supporting the validity of the typical patterns found. Further, it was shown that cluster membership contributed to the prediction
of some validation variables above the prediction achieved by using only SWB components entered as continuous variables, suggesting
the presence of interactions and nonlinearities in the SWB area. 相似文献
147.
Kompus K Specht K Ersland L Juvodden HT van Wageningen H Hugdahl K Westerhausen R 《Brain and language》2012,121(3):240-247
We report fMRI and behavioral data from 113 subjects on attention and cognitive control using a variant of the classic dichotic listening paradigm with pairwise presentations of consonant-vowel syllables. The syllable stimuli were presented in a block-design while subjects were in the MR scanner. The subjects were instructed to pay attention to and report either the left or right ear stimulus. The hypothesis was that paying attention to the left ear stimulus (FL condition) induces a cognitive conflict, requiring cognitive control processes, not seen when paying attention to the right ear stimulus (FR condition), due to the perceptual salience of the right ear stimulus in a dichotic situation. The FL condition resulted in distinct activations in the left inferior prefrontal gyrus and caudate nucleus, while the right inferior frontal gyrus and caudate were activated in both the FL and FR conditions, and in a non-instructed (NF) baseline condition. 相似文献
148.
Strother L Kubovy M 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2006,32(2):226-234
The authors conducted 3 experiments to explore the roles of curvature, density, and relative proximity in the perceptual organization of ambiguous dot patterns. To this end, they developed a new family of regular dot patterns that tend to be perceptually grouped into parallel contours, dot-sampled structured grids (DSGs). DSGs are similar to the dot lattices used to study grouping by proximity, except that only one of the potential organizations is rectilinear; the others are curvilinear. The authors used the method of M. Kubovy and J. Wagemans (1995) to study grouping by proximity in DSGs. They found that in the competition between the most likely organizations, one rectilinear and the other curvilinear, the latter is more salient. This phenomenon cannot be explained by contemporary accounts of grouping by proximity or contour integration. 相似文献
149.
150.
Research suggests that the striatum plays an important role in implicit learning (IL). The striatum exhibits marked age-related
morphological and neurochemical losses. Yet, behavioral studies suggest that IL is generally well preserved in old age, and
that age-related differences emerge only when highly complex IL tasks are used. In this review, we integrate behavioral and
neuroimaging evidence on IL in aging. We suggest that relative stability of IL in old age may reflect neural reorganization
that compensates for age-related losses in striatal functions. Specifically, there may be an age-related increase in reliance
on extrastriatal regions (e.g., medial-temporal, frontal) during IL. This reorganization of function may be beneficial under
less taxing performance conditions, but not when task demands become more challenging. 相似文献