首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   840篇
  免费   26篇
  2020年   7篇
  2019年   5篇
  2018年   15篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   81篇
  2012年   27篇
  2011年   25篇
  2010年   24篇
  2009年   13篇
  2008年   28篇
  2007年   27篇
  2006年   32篇
  2005年   29篇
  2004年   35篇
  2003年   34篇
  2002年   27篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   17篇
  1999年   24篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   14篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   13篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   16篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   10篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   16篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   17篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   13篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   9篇
  1976年   12篇
  1975年   9篇
  1974年   10篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   5篇
  1966年   3篇
排序方式: 共有866条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
A theory of how connections between working memory (Science 255 (1992) 556; in: G. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 8, Academic Press, New York, p. 47) and cognitive functions of the cerebellum (Trends Neurosci 16(11) (1993) 448; Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. 9 (1999) 718; Behav. Neurosci. 100 (1986) 443, Behav. Neurosci. 103 (1989) 998) lead to mathematical discovery is presented. It is proposed that (a) patterns of repetitious working memory processing are formed in the cerebellum, and (b) when these cerebellar patterns are subsequently fed back to control processing in working memory, they may become cognized in visuospatial imagery and language as the concepts and axioms that underlie mathematical discovery. It is concluded that a neurophysiological explanation of the cognitive origins of mathematics (L. English (Ed.), Mathematical Reasoning: Analogies, Metaphors, and images, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, p. 21, where Mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being, Basic Books, New York) can be based upon how conceptual constructions arise from the collaborative interactions of working memory and the cognitive functions of the cerebellum.  相似文献   
122.
123.
124.
125.
126.
127.
128.
The effects and side effects of overcorrection for self-stimulatory behaviors of two children in a specialized day-care program were evaluated. For one child, a “hand” overcorrection procedure involving arm and hand exercises was introduced contingent upon inappropriate hand movements and later contingent upon inappropriate foot movements. After “hand” overcorrection was withdrawn for inappropriate foot movements, a “foot” overcorrection procedure involving foot and leg exercises was introduced contingent upon inappropriate foot movements. For a second child, the “hand” overcorrection procedure was introduced contingent upon inappropriate hand movements during a free-play period, and later contingent upon inappropriate vocalizations at naptime. “Hand” overcorrection was withdrawn and then re-introduced sequentially for both behaviors. Several concurrent behaviors were measured to assess multiple effects of treatment. Results for both children indicated the “hand” overcorrection procedure suppressed inappropriate hand movements and inappropriate behaviors that were topographically dissimilar. In addition, inverse relationships were observed between the second child's inappropriate hand movements and appropriate toy usage during free play and between his inappropriate vocalizations and inappropriate foot movements during naptime. Results suggest that overcorrection procedures that are effective for one behavior can be used to reduce the frequency of topographically different behaviors. This finding is discussed in terms of its practical implications for therapists.  相似文献   
129.
The encoding of either physical or semantic features of words was biased in an intentional learning situation. A modified recognition test was then employed to assess the effectiveness of this study manipulation and its consequences for retention. The Ss were required to select test items that were either physically similar, semantically similar, or identical to a study word. Results revealed that Ss biased toward physical encoding were more successful in selecting physically similar than semantically similar test items, while the opposite was true of Ss biased toward semantic encoding. The Ss in the two study conditions did not differ in their ability to select test items that were identical to a study word. This pattern of results was interpreted as evidence that semantic and physical information can be equally well retained over the long term. Limits on the generality of prior findings of rapid decay for physical information are discussed.  相似文献   
130.
Twelve pigeons were given successive discrimination training involving variable-interval reinforcement for key pecking in the presence of one intensity of monochromatic light randomly alternated with extinction for pecking during another intensity. All of the pigeons were then tested in extinction for generalization along the intensity dimension, and all showed a displacement of maximal responding from the positive stimulus in the direction opposite the negative stimulus. For six of the pigeons, for which the test included only three values beyond the positive stimulus, four showed peaked gradients but two did not, showing monotonic gradients with maximal responding to the most extreme test value. For another six pigeons tested over a wider range, all showed peaked gradients. Thus, when a sufficiently wide range of test values is employed, generalization gradients for visual intensity have the same peaked form as do gradients for qualitative visual dimensions such as wavelength or line angle.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号