首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   992篇
  免费   38篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   12篇
  2018年   23篇
  2017年   20篇
  2016年   22篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   105篇
  2012年   32篇
  2011年   32篇
  2010年   24篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   29篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   37篇
  2005年   32篇
  2004年   36篇
  2003年   36篇
  2002年   34篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   27篇
  1998年   19篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   15篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   15篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   13篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   15篇
  1982年   16篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   16篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   15篇
  1975年   11篇
  1974年   11篇
  1971年   6篇
  1966年   6篇
  1965年   5篇
排序方式: 共有1030条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
141.
142.
Inadequate patient adherence to treatment regimens is a ubiquitous problem in health care and carries a profound personal, societal, and economic cost. This article illustrates a general theoretical framework we believe to be useful for the interpretation, conception, and design of adherence research. The core tenet of this framework is that factors that influence adherence can be better understood by considering the interactive effects of patients' characteristics, type of adherence intervention, and characteristics of the illness and medical treatment context. This framework represents an extension and application of previous theory and research from personality, social, and clinical psychology concerning the value of an interactionalist perspective. We illustrate the framework using some of our past work involving treatment adherence among patients with chronic renal failure.  相似文献   
143.
144.
A theory of how connections between working memory (Science 255 (1992) 556; in: G. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 8, Academic Press, New York, p. 47) and cognitive functions of the cerebellum (Trends Neurosci 16(11) (1993) 448; Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. 9 (1999) 718; Behav. Neurosci. 100 (1986) 443, Behav. Neurosci. 103 (1989) 998) lead to mathematical discovery is presented. It is proposed that (a) patterns of repetitious working memory processing are formed in the cerebellum, and (b) when these cerebellar patterns are subsequently fed back to control processing in working memory, they may become cognized in visuospatial imagery and language as the concepts and axioms that underlie mathematical discovery. It is concluded that a neurophysiological explanation of the cognitive origins of mathematics (L. English (Ed.), Mathematical Reasoning: Analogies, Metaphors, and images, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, p. 21, where Mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being, Basic Books, New York) can be based upon how conceptual constructions arise from the collaborative interactions of working memory and the cognitive functions of the cerebellum.  相似文献   
145.
146.
147.
How do people come up with humorous ideas? In creative cognition research, exposure to good examples sometimes causes fixation (people get “stuck” on the examples) but other times sparks inspiration (people's responses are more creative). The present research examined the effects of funny and unfunny examples on joke production. A sample of 175 adults read scenarios that they completed with funny responses. All participants were instructed to be funny, but before responding they read (a) funny responses as examples of good responses to emulate, (b) unfunny responses as examples of poor responses to avoid, or (c) no examples. The participants’ own responses were rated for funniness and for similarity to the example responses, and response times were recorded. Reading either funny or unfunny examples, compared to no examples, caused people to come up with funnier jokes. Similarity to the examples was low in all conditions, so fixation was relatively modest, but people who saw unfunny examples spent more time coming up with their responses. Taken together, the findings support the growing literature showing that examples are often inspiring rather than constraining, and they imply that good and bad examples spark creative thought via different paths.  相似文献   
148.
149.
150.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号