全文获取类型
收费全文 | 133篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
133篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 14篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 8篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 1篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有133条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
This study employs a secondary analysis of U.S. nationally representative data from the Pew Internet 2008 civic engagement survey (N?=?2251) to examine the degree to which contacting public officials both online and offline is explained by the variables of gender and political connectedness. We find that while women are somewhat less likely to contact public officials through direct means, such as emailing or writing a letter, they are more likely to sign petitions, offline and especially online. Gender gaps in direct forms of contact are smaller in the online context than the offline context. We additionally find that that gender moderates the relationship between political connectedness developed via social networking sites and contacting public officials, such that women gain even further advantage in signing online petitions, but also gain further disadvantage in writing a letter/calling public officials and signing offline petitions. Finally, we find that political connectedness, achieved via social networking sites and offline contexts, is related to contacting public officials through both online and traditional means, suggesting a blurring of offline and online worlds. 相似文献
92.
Windmann S Schneider T Reczio J Grobosch M Voelzke V Blasius V Brämer A Ischebeck W Janikowski G Mandrella W Unger C Wischnjak L 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2006,6(4):270-276
In recognition memory tasks, emotionally negative words are judged more often as "old" relative to emotionally neutral words, suggesting a shift in response bias. We wondered whether this bias shift was due to the flexible regulation of executive control during memory retrieval. To address this question, we investigated individuals with high variability in executive control functions. As expected, we observed that emotional word meaning did indeed have a strong influence on the bias toward responding "old," independent of recognition accuracy and overall response bias. However, these effects were uncorrelated with executive control, as measured by the Trail Making Test, and were fully intact, even in a sample of hospitalized neurological patients with severe executive dysfunctions, some of whom had marked damage in fronto-thalamo-striatal networks. Having concluded that the emotion-induced bias must develop on different grounds, we went on to discuss alternative explanations. 相似文献
93.
It is well established that goals energize and direct behaviour across the lifespan. To better understand how goals are embedded in people's lives across adulthood, the present research examined life goals' content (health, personal growth, prosocial engagement, social relations, status, work), dynamics (interplay between goal importance and goal attainability), and outcomes (subjective well‐being) from a developmental perspective. We argue that people rate those goals as important and attainable that enable them to master developmental tasks, that they adapt their goals to personal capacities, and that goals predict subjective well‐being after 2 and 4 years. The sample included 973 individuals (18–92 years old, M = 43.00 years) of whom 637 participated 2 years later and 573 participated 4 years later. Goal importance and well‐being were assessed at all occasions and goal attainability at the first two occasions. Results indicated that age was negatively associated with importance and attainability of personal‐growth, status, and work goals but positively associated with importance and attainability of prosocial‐engagement goals. The association between goal importance and attainability was largely bidirectional over time; and goal attainability, rather than goal importance, was positively linked to later well‐being. Implications of these findings are discussed in light of adult lifespan development. © 2019 European Association of Personality Psychology 相似文献
94.
Tana L. Hope Christina Adams Larissa Reynolds Denise Powers Rose Ann Perez Mary Lou Kelley 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1999,21(4):349-363
Assessment of psychopathology in adolescents can be complicated due to uncertainties about who should be considered the primary informant. While a multimethod, multiinformant approach to assessment allows for a thorough assessment, it can also result in contradictory findings. The purpose of this study was to use a sample of 121 adolescents to investigate issues of parent-adolescent agreement on behavior rating scales and a structured diagnostic interview. Additionally, this study evaluated whether adolescent report offers unique information to the assessment beyond the parent's report. The sample included both normal controls and adolescents who met criteria for diagnosis of an Internalizing or Externalizing disorder based on the DSM-IV criteria. Agreement between parents and adolescents on the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents—Revised (DICA-R) was low for ADHD (39.4%) and Externalizing Disorders (41.1%) and moderate for Internalizing Disorders (56.6%). A significant difference was found between the number of ADHD symptoms reported, with parents reporting more symptoms (M = 9.42) than adolescents (M = 8.34). Regression analysis indicated that the adolescent completed Youth Self-report (YSR) contributed unique information beyond the parent completed measures in predicting both the adolescent and the parent interviews for Internalizing Disorders. This finding emphasizes the need to continue to collect self-report information, especially when assessing Internalizing disorders, which inherently contain symptoms indicating high-risk behaviors such as suicide ideation. 相似文献
95.
Larissa G. Duncan J. Douglas Coatsworth Mark T. Greenberg 《Clinical child and family psychology review》2009,12(3):255-270
This paper introduces a model of “mindful parenting” as a framework whereby parents intentionally bring moment-to-moment awareness
to the parent–child relationship. This is done by developing the qualities of listening with full attention when interacting
with their children, cultivating emotional awareness and self-regulation in parenting, and bringing compassion and nonjudgmental
acceptance to their parenting interactions. First, we briefly outline the theoretical and empirical literature on mindfulness
and mindfulness-based interventions. Next, we present an operational definition of mindful parenting as an extension of mindfulness
to the social context of parent–child relationships. We discuss the implications of mindful parenting for the quality of parent–child
relationships, particularly across the transition to adolescence, and we review the literature on the application of mindfulness
in parenting interventions. We close with a synopsis of our own efforts to integrate mindfulness-based intervention techniques
and mindful parenting into a well-established, evidence-based family prevention program and our recommendations for future
research on mindful parenting interventions. 相似文献
96.
This article is an effort to refine current understanding of social integration in the context of repatriate migration. The
empirical basis for this analysis is a recent survey in a national sample of post-1990 Russian Jewish immigrants to Israel
that examined the pace and determinants of their integration and acculturation. The proposed analytical framework in based
on the four main indicators of integration among educated first-genaration immigrants: (a) the improving command of the host
language and emerging bilingualism; (b) skilled occupation in the mainstream economy; (c) diversification of immigrants’ informal
networks to include members of the host society; and (d) shifts in the cultural and media consumption from coethnic (i.e.,
Russian-based) to the mainstream (i.e., Hebrew/English-based) products. The study helps to sketch social profiles of the immigrants
prone to social integration versus separatism; it also taps into the issue of cultural contact between the newcomers and non-immigrant
Israelis measured on both sides of theencounter. 相似文献
97.
Mariëlle E. Abrahamse PhD Larissa N. Niec PhD David T. Solomon PhD Marianne Junger PhD Ramón J. L. Lindauer MD PhD 《Child & family behavior therapy》2013,35(3):141-158
AbstractThe current study explored the psychometric properties of a parent-child interaction observation system, the Dyadic Parent-Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS) in the Netherlands. Participants included 31 Dutch mother-child dyads and 86?U.S. mother-child dyads for a community sample (2–7?years; 50% boys). Good one-week test-retest reliability was demonstrated among the Dutch sample. Similarities were found between Dutch and U.S. samples on most interaction codes, but mothers in the U.S. sample used more directive behaviors (e.g., commands) in some situations. Findings suggest that the DPICS is a reliable measure of mother-child interactions in the Dutch population. Cultural issues regarding the use of the DPICS are discussed. 相似文献
98.
The aim of this study was to examine whether shyness is a risk factor for second language acquisition in immigrant preschoolers. Results from studies on first language acquisition indicate that shy children show less favorable language development; however, it remains unclear how shyness affects second language acquisition. As second language skills are often acquired in interactions outside the family where shyness is more evident, we postulate that shyness has a strong negative effect on second language acquisition. This hypothesis was examined using standardized tests and parental ratings in a sample of 330 immigrant preschoolers cross-sectionally and with 130 immigrant preschoolers longitudinally. The analyses revealed lower second language competence as well as slower language development in shy immigrant children compared to their non-shy counterparts. The present study highlights that not only contextual but also personality factors need to be considered for a comprehensive understanding of second language acquisition in immigrant children. 相似文献
99.
Mathias Kauff;Lena Lämmle;Esther Kroll;Larissa Gehring;Roman Soucek;Frank Asbrock; 《Journal of community & applied social psychology》2024,34(4):e2867
Expanding upon Duckitt's dual-process motivational model of ideology and prejudice (DPM; Duckitt, 2001), this research explores the psychological underpinnings of the approval of hostile design (AoHD) measures. Hostile design measures are interventions designed to deter undesirable behaviours and specific social groups in urban areas. Across three correlational studies (N = 798) conducted in Germany and the US, we investigated the relationship between AoHD, Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), Social Dominance Orientation (SDO), as well as dangerous and competitive worldviews. AoHD was positively associated with RWA and SDO, as well as with dangerous and competitive worldviews. In line with the DPM, the relationship between a competitive worldview and AoHD is mediated by SDO in Studies 2 and 3. In addition, the relationship between a dangerous worldview and AoHD is mediated by RWA in Study 2 but not in Study 3. Please refer to the Supplementary Material section to find this article's Community and Social Impact Statement. 相似文献
100.
Larissa N. Niec Jenelle R. Shanley Miya L. Barnett Sarah E. Baker David T. Solomon 《Child & family behavior therapy》2015,37(2):105-113
The behavioral assessment of parent-child interactions is an important component of treatment planning and evaluation in many evidence-based parent training programs. However, numerous factors unrelated to the parent-child interaction may add error to the assessment. This study investigated the impact of task instructions on parents’ behaviors during the analogue parent-child interaction observation, the Dyadic Parent-Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS). Forty-eight mother-child dyads were randomly assigned to one of two conditions that differed on level of specificity of instructions. Significant differences were observed between groups, with parents who received specific instructions using more praises during the DPICS tasks. Specific instructions may shift parents’ behaviors toward optimal, rather than typical, interaction patterns and should be considered in the context of the assessment goals. 相似文献