全文获取类型
收费全文 | 59313篇 |
免费 | 2401篇 |
国内免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
61732篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 557篇 |
2019年 | 741篇 |
2018年 | 987篇 |
2017年 | 1016篇 |
2016年 | 1083篇 |
2015年 | 787篇 |
2014年 | 927篇 |
2013年 | 4335篇 |
2012年 | 1744篇 |
2011年 | 1905篇 |
2010年 | 1171篇 |
2009年 | 1171篇 |
2008年 | 1681篇 |
2007年 | 1700篇 |
2006年 | 1554篇 |
2005年 | 1364篇 |
2004年 | 1266篇 |
2003年 | 1186篇 |
2002年 | 1294篇 |
2001年 | 1963篇 |
2000年 | 1940篇 |
1999年 | 1465篇 |
1998年 | 684篇 |
1997年 | 610篇 |
1996年 | 610篇 |
1993年 | 548篇 |
1992年 | 1240篇 |
1991年 | 1139篇 |
1990年 | 1125篇 |
1989年 | 1028篇 |
1988年 | 1009篇 |
1987年 | 964篇 |
1986年 | 1041篇 |
1985年 | 1046篇 |
1984年 | 877篇 |
1983年 | 799篇 |
1982年 | 559篇 |
1981年 | 557篇 |
1979年 | 934篇 |
1978年 | 665篇 |
1975年 | 762篇 |
1974年 | 817篇 |
1973年 | 909篇 |
1972年 | 766篇 |
1971年 | 722篇 |
1970年 | 642篇 |
1969年 | 668篇 |
1968年 | 856篇 |
1967年 | 777篇 |
1966年 | 651篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 9 毫秒
841.
M. J. Morgan 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(4):663-673
In a fixed interval schedule of reinforcement the only responses to be reinforced are those made when a certain time interval has elapsed since the previous reinforcement. The behaviour of three rats on such a schedule was compared with their behaviour on a schedule where a response made at any time during the interval was reinforced by setting up a reward which was delivered when the interval had elapsed. Response rates were higher in the ordinary fixed interval schedule than in its modified version, and it is argued that this rules out attempts to explain the maintenance of fixed interval performance by delayed reinforcement. Despite the clear difference in response rates, there was considerable similarity between the post-reinforcement pauses developed in the two schedules, and this suggests that pausing is influenced more by temporal than by response contingencies. 相似文献
842.
G. R. J. Hockey 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(1):37-42
It was previously found that noise increased both performance on a primary tracking task and the detection of centrally located signals in a secondary multi-source monitoring task. The present experiment examines the effect of changing the distribution of signals across the monitoring display. When equal numbers are seen at all locations there is no differential effect of noise for central and peripheral locations. Such an effect only occurs when central signals are seen to have greater probability. This result points to the selectivity effect in noise being a function of task priorities and not of physical location. 相似文献
843.
Sidney Stecher Michael Sandberg Paul J. Minsky 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1970,7(2):79-85
Monocular,successive luminance difference thresholds (ΔI) and brightness matches (PSE) were obtained by the method of constant stimuli for two flashes successively presented to the same retinal area. Variations in interstimulus interval first-flash duration,and second-flash duration were the independent variables investigated. ΔI decreased as a function of ISI,while PSE remained relatively uninfluenced. An intensity-duration reciprocity was observed with increases in either first- or second-flash duration. Equal increases in duration of both flashes led to a constant value of ΔI. A Broca-Sulzer effect was also noted. In another study, a 10-msec, variable-luminance Standard was followed after 500 msec by either a 10-msec or a 320-msec test flash that was compared to the Standard. The results indicated that the rate of change of brightness with changes in luminance of the Standard was faster for the 10-msec flash than for the 320-msec flash. The rate-of-change hypothesis would predict that the 10-msec flash should have the smaller ΔI. The results for two Ss indicated the opposite: AI was smaller for 320-msec than for 10-msec flashes. A modification of the hypothesis was suggested such that it may be the energy increment (Δlt) required for detection that is related to the rate at which brightness changes with energy. 相似文献
844.
Effects of a conditioned reinforcer upon accuracy of match-to-sample behavior in pigeons 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Three pigeons were trained to perform a two-key sequential match-to-sample task. During baseline conditions, food reinforcement was contingent upon the first match response to occur following 8-min periods, and orange illumination of both keys preceded the delivery of food by 0.5 sec. The baseline schedule of food reinforcement was in effect throughout the study. In some conditions, a 0.5-sec flash of orange keylight alone was presented contingent upon mismatch responses that followed variable time periods averaging 1 min. Rate of mismatch responses increased and accuracy of matching performances decreased as compared with baseline conditions. The ability of the 0.5-sec orange flash to reinforce mismatch responses was markedly reduced when it no longer immediately preceded the delivery of food. 相似文献
845.
Responding under fixed-ratio and multiple fixed-interval fixed-ratio schedules of electric shock presentation 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Squirrel monkeys, initially trained under a schedule of electric shock postponement and then under fixed-interval schedules of electric shock presentation, were studied under multiple fixed-interval fixed-ratio and under fixed-ratio schedules of shock presentation. Under the fixed-interval (10-min) component of the multiple schedule, a pause was followed by a gradual increase in responding to a rate maintained until shock presentation; under the fixed-ratio (3-, 10-, or 30-response) component of the multiple schedule, a brief pause was typically followed by a relatively high and uniform rate of responding until shock was presented. When the 60-sec timeout periods, which usually followed shock presentation, were eliminated from the multiple schedule for one monkey, responding was only transiently affected. In the one monkey studied, responding was maintained under a fixed-ratio schedule alone (with timeout periods), but rates of responding were lower than under the fixed-ratio component of the multiple schedule. Characteristic patterns of responding, similar to those engendered under schedules of food presentation or shock termination, can be maintained under fixed-ratio schedules of shock presentation; further, patterns of responding can be controlled by discriminative stimuli in multiple schedules. 相似文献
846.
Nine subjects, 14 to 18 yr old, pulled a knob on a schedule of monetary reinforcement. Concurrently, they escaped or avoided periodic presentations of a tone by pressing a button that required 1.5 lb (6.67 N) of force or by punching a padded cushion that required 20 lb (88.96 N) of force. The punching response was designated as an aggressive response because the force of this response together with its topography was comparable to responses of humans that deface objects and produce escape or counter aggression from other humans. It was found that button pressing was the preferred concurrent avoidance response and there were few punches. However, when the monetary reinforcer was discontinued (extinction) punching increased for seven of the nine subjects, but there was no consistent change in the rate of button pressing. When the punching response was replaced by another non-preferred but non-aggressive response, neither this response nor button pressing increased during extinction. Hence, the increase in punching during extinction cannot be attributed solely to the fact that it was a concurrent response or a non-preferred response. 相似文献
847.
Personality-trait descriptions of differentially liked persons 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
848.
Recovery from surgery, with and without preparatory instruction, for three coping styles 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
J M Andrew 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1970,15(3):223-226
849.
850.