首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   679篇
  免费   13篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   16篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   20篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   71篇
  2012年   31篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   14篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   30篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   27篇
  2004年   19篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   25篇
  2001年   13篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   15篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   9篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   10篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   8篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   8篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   9篇
  1975年   4篇
  1973年   9篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   7篇
  1970年   4篇
  1969年   6篇
  1968年   8篇
  1967年   7篇
排序方式: 共有692条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
In recent studies of the structure of affect, positive and negative affect have consistently emerged as two dominant and relatively independent dimensions. A number of mood scales have been created to measure these factors; however, many existing measures are inadequate, showing low reliability or poor convergent or discriminant validity. To fill the need for reliable and valid Positive Affect and Negative Affect scales that are also brief and easy to administer, we developed two 10-item mood scales that comprise the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). The scales are shown to be highly internally consistent, largely uncorrelated, and stable at appropriate levels over a 2-month time period. Normative data and factorial and external evidence of convergent and discriminant validity for the scales are also presented.  相似文献   
152.
Three groups of subjects were administered a redefinition strategy which asked them to focus on sensations during noxious stimulation. Those in one group were informed that the strategy would reduce pain (positive expectancy), those in a second were informed that it would augment pain (negative expectancy), and those in a third were given no information on expectancy. Subjects in a fourth group received neither redefinition nor instructions on expectancy. The four groups did not differ either in expectancy of pain reduction or in reduction of reported pain. Subjects in all groups expected more pain than they reported, and expectations for pain reduction showed only a small correlation with degree of reported pain reduction. In all groups, men reported less pain than women.  相似文献   
153.
This study examined the relationship between gender, sex role, and narcissism. Two hundred and three students completed the Bem Sex Role Inventory, the Narcissistic Personality Inventory, and the Narcissistic Personality Disorder Scale, along with several measures of self-esteem and depression. Overall, the data indicated that males and masculine individuals were not higher in their levels of maladaptive narcissism, that an adjusted narcissism was more obvious in males and in masculine subjects, and that femininity appeared to inhibit the display of an unhealthy exploitive self-concern. Androgyny failed to appear as the healthiest sex role, although multiple regression analyses suggested that future research may need to further explore this possibility.  相似文献   
154.
The effects of four doses of chlorpromazine (dose range 0.5 to 12.5 mg/kg) on performance under a delayed matching-to-sample procedure in pigeons was investigated, using the exponential model of memory (White, 1985). Performance was measured using a bias-free measure of discriminability, log d (Davison & Tustin, 1978), and negative exponential functions were fitted to individual-subject and group data at each dose level. A decrease in matching accuracy was found to be caused by an increase in the rate of forgetting, b, and a decrease in the initial discriminability, log d0. Changes in rate of forgetting and discriminability occurred at doses that had no statistically significant effect on response latency. The exponential model of memory accounted well for the data and provided a useful way of quantifying the effects of chlorpromazine on the processes involved in delayed matching-to-sample performance.  相似文献   
155.
Diurnal variation in the Positive Affects   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
We examined the circadian rhythm of two basic dimensions of mood—Positive Affect (PA) and Negative Affect (NA). Subjects were 196 college students in two studies who completed a mood rating form approximately seven times a day for 1 week. Significant diurnal variation was found to exist in overall Positive Affect, and in four component content areas, but not in Negative Affect. Specifically, all components of PA rose sharply from early morning until noon, remained relatively constant until 9 p.m., and then fell rapidly. The rise and fall of PA were quite robust across subjects, but the apparently static level of PA from noon to 9 p.m. was shown to result from averaging across marked individual differences in the time of peak PA. Various potential sources of these individual differences—“morningness-eveningness,” introversion-extraversion, and neuroticism/dysphoria—were investigated. Neither of the personality variables accounted for the significant variation in diurnal mood patterning, but clearly defined morning and evening types did show different patterns. Possible links between the mood pattern obtained and biologically based circadian rhythms, which have been postulated to be dysregulated in depression and mania, are discussed.  相似文献   
156.
This study, using an ABABA design, investigated the effects of a self-monitoring (SM) procedure on the sustained schoolwork (SS) behavior of a four-year-old child. The results indicated that SM resulted in an increased rate of SS behavior in the absence of a clear functional relation between ecological variables (e.g., teacher attention) and SS behavior. Increased SS was accompanied by an increase in compliance (C) behavior. The observed co-variation between SS and C was discussed in terms of Wahler's (1975) response-class hypothesis.  相似文献   
157.
158.
159.
160.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号