全文获取类型
收费全文 | 908篇 |
免费 | 19篇 |
专业分类
927篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 31篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 23篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 92篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 41篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 23篇 |
2008年 | 33篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 26篇 |
2004年 | 23篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 33篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 14篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 6篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有927条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
901.
Rosemary Anne Arthur James Fitzwater Ross Roberts James Hardy Calum Alexander Arthur 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2017,29(4):449-465
We examined the indirect effects of basic psychological skills (PS) on military endurance through enhanced advanced PS while controlling for fitness. British Army recruits (n = 159) participated in three endurance events for Parachute Regiment selection and completed an adapted Test of Performance Strategies questionnaire (Hardy et al., 2010). Following confirmatory factor analyses, the multiple mediation regression analyses using PROCESS (Hayes, 2013) suggested that goal setting, imagery and relaxation all had positive indirect effects on endurance via activation, with goal setting also impacting on endurance via negative thinking. These data provide some support for basic PS influencing endurance via advanced PS. 相似文献
902.
Research on implicit and explicit attitudes towards those with a visible difference has provided contradictory findings. While studies suggest explicit attitudes towards those with a visible difference are positive, implicit attitudes are likely to be negative. An Implicit Association Test measured attitudes towards visible differences in 129 adults finding no evidence of negative implicit attitudes. This contrasts with previous findings and accounts of those with a visible difference who report perceiving negative behaviours by the general public. Results are discussed in terms of what is known about perceiving faces that are different, as well as socio-cultural changes. These findings provide preliminary evidence that implicit attitudes towards visible difference are not negative, and that behaviour may be a result of uncertainty regarding how to behave. 相似文献
903.
904.
Trevor G. Mazzucchelli Emma Burton Lynne Roberts 《Australian journal of psychology》2019,71(3):305-311
905.
Investigators sometimes use timelines (visual depictions of time) to help children identify temporal information from experienced events or details from a particular instance of a repeated event. However, little is actually known about the efficacy of this visual aid on children's memories. Six‐ to 9‐year‐olds participated in four occurrences of a repeated event and were later interviewed with or without the use of a timeline. Children in the timeline condition were less accurate and made more source‐monitoring errors than those in the control condition. For items that appeared consistently throughout the events, a suggestibility effect was more evident for older children who used the timeline than those who did not. Overall, there was no clear evidence in support of the visual aid. Despite its popularity, our findings suggest that timelines should be further examined to establish whether, and under what circumstances, they might be used in investigative contexts. 相似文献
906.
Kelly Morris Roberts 《Reflective Practice》2019,20(2):201-217
This paper analyzes the results of an 18 month study of a variety of participants involved in written reflection on their learning, both in classes at two universities in the Southern US or in voluntary focus groups. The purpose of the study was to analyze the effects of including autobiographical tools in written reflection. Participants used three tools from the genre of autobiography as they reflected on their learning inside either course content, internship experiences, experiential learning, or goal-setting. Over 500 samples were gathered, offering a large data set for analysis of what kinds of best practice produce quality reflective writing. Through analysis of the data and student surveys and interviews, results show increased attention to critical thinking and metacognition; expressed flexibility, adaptability, and ability to handle ambiguity; and value in connection to others/empathy-building as a result of incorporating autobiographical tools in reflective prompts. The study indicated that reflections using the same autobiographical tool over the course of several sessions seemed to produce deeper levels of reflection and more engagement with the autobiographical tool. Of the three autobiographical tools used in the study, the tool that seemed most helpful to participants was reflection as storytelling that revealed aspects of the self. 相似文献
907.
908.
Participants (6- and 7-year-olds, N=130) participated in classroom activities four times. Children were interviewed about the final occurrence (target event) either 1 week or 4 weeks later, during which half of the event items were described inaccurately. Half of these suggestions were consistent with the theme of the detail across the occurrences (e.g., always sat on a kind of floor mat) or were inconsistent (e.g., sat on a chair). When memory for the target event was tested 1 day later, children falsely recognized fewer inconsistent suggestions than consistent suggestions, especially compared with a control group of children who experienced the event just one time. Furthermore, the longer delay reduced accuracy only for consistent suggestions. Source-monitoring ability was strongly and positively related to resistance to suggestions, and encouraging children to identify the source of false suggestions allowed them to retract a significant proportion of their reports of inconsistent suggestions but not of consistent suggestions. The results suggest that the gist consistency of suggestions determines whether event repetition increases or decreases suggestibility. 相似文献
909.
910.
Like mother,like daughter: similarities in narrative style 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Children (2-5-year-olds and 8-13-year-olds) and their parents were independently interviewed about highly salient events: injuries serious enough to necessitate hospital emergency room treatment. Free recall narratives were scored using 14 measures of length, elaborative detail, cohesion, coherence,and provision of context. Mothers' narratives were more cohesive and coherent than fathers', and girls' narratives differed from boys' in parallel ways. Parent and child measures were correlated, and narratives of mother-daughter dyads (for the older daughters) showed striking similarity in all 5 properties, whereas there was no narrative similarity within father-son, mother-son, or father-daughter dyads. This suggests a special status for mother-daughter dyads in terms of how events come to be linguistically represented in narrative. 相似文献