首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   871篇
  免费   49篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   10篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   23篇
  2020年   29篇
  2019年   13篇
  2018年   37篇
  2017年   46篇
  2016年   46篇
  2015年   27篇
  2014年   25篇
  2013年   129篇
  2012年   58篇
  2011年   62篇
  2010年   39篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   41篇
  2007年   34篇
  2006年   29篇
  2005年   25篇
  2004年   23篇
  2003年   35篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   6篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   5篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   3篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   4篇
  1970年   3篇
  1968年   3篇
排序方式: 共有921条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
The model of commitment presented in the present report postulates personality to be the crucial element in the development and maintenance of commitment to the profession of art. The objectives of the research reported here were to (a) demonstrate the typicality of the artist personality and (b) identify the traits which characterize committed and uncommitted artists. Three groups of artists were compared: mature established artists described as committed over a lifetime, committed art students, and uncommitted art students. The instrument used to evaluate personality was the Adjective Check List (ACL; Gough 8c Heilbrun, 1983). Both committed and uncommitted artists presented a typical personality structure; but in contrast to the committed, the uncommitted were characterized by low self‐esteem, a negative self‐image, impaired motivation, low staying power, and an inability to find a clear direction. Both art students and mature artists agreed about the order of importance of 11 factors considered to be determinants of committment. They both placed particular emphasis on autonomy, possibility to experiment, ease of inspiration, and early identification with the profession.  相似文献   
72.
One hundred fifty-one children and 43 adults judged which of 2 cartoon birds would be the first to arrive at a common finish line. Objects moved unidirectionally along parallel trajectories, either at the same or different speeds, and disappeared at different distances from the goal. Overall, 9–10-year-old children performed as well as adults, but 4–5- and 6–8-year-olds erred significantly more often. On trials for which distance to goal at disappearance was a valid cue, 4–5-year-olds scored 80% correct, and no differences were seen between 6–10-year-olds and adults. On the opposite type of trials, where the trailing bird would win the race, only adults retained their level of performance, and all age groups differed markedly. Findings suggest a gradual developmental transition from a distance-based to a time-based understanding of the task.  相似文献   
73.
In a relatively short time span, issues of ethnic, cultural, and religious diversity have become central topics of discussion in various nations. As a result, the role of broad ideologies that frame and structure relations between groups has received increasing attention by social psychologists. Of particular concern has been the role of these intergroup ideologies in promoting intergroup harmony and reducing prejudice. In this article, we appraise the evidence related to three main intergroup ideologies, assimilation, colourblindness, and multiculturalism. We argue that research in this area has paid insufficient attention to the social and political context. Intergroup ideologies have been studied and conceptualised as being located solely within individual minds. We suggest that the potentially vital aspect of these ideologies is that they are sometimes widely shared by members of a social group. Integrating sociological and political analyses, we discuss the fact that intergroup ideologies are institutionalised as policies and that, as such, they often vary across countries and across time. We present a series of studies to illustrate the theoretical implications of studying the shared nature of these intergroup ideologies, providing insights into the question of when and why national policies can shape individuals’ intergroup attitudes and beliefs and improve intergroup relations.  相似文献   
74.
(Geldard, Frank A. The Human Senses. New York: Wiley, 1953. Pp. 365.) Reviewed by John Paul Nafe.  相似文献   
75.
By using inverse dynamics and forceplate recordings, this study established the principle of oscillating systems and the influence of gravity and body parameters on the programming of the gait parameters, step frequency and length. Calculation of the ratio of the amplitude of the center of mass (CM) and the center of foot pressure (CP) oscillations yielded an equation and established a biomechanical constant, the natural body frequency (NBF). NBF appears to be an absolute invariant parameter, specific to human standing posture and gait in terrestrial gravity, which influences the relative positions of CM and CP and whose value separates the frequency bands of standing posture from those for gait. This equation was tested by using the experimental paradigm of stepping in place and then used in calculating the magnitude of CM oscillations during gait. The biomechanical analysis of the experimental observations allows one to establish the relationships between body parameters and gravity and the central programming of locomotor parameters.  相似文献   
76.
This article describes developmental changes in gait velocity and relates these changes to gait parameters that index postural stability (step width and lateral acceleration) and two components of velocity (cadence and step length).

Five children were observed longitudinally over a 2-year period after onset of independent walking. Their range of speed increased threefold in the first 6 months of independent walking and then remained constant. In contrast, step width decreased approximately twofold. Whereas in adults, cadence and step length contribute approximately equally to speed, when infants first begin to walk independently, increase in velocity is due mostly to increased step length. After 5 months of independent walking, the pattern reverses, and increase in velocity is due primarly to increased cadence. The pattern remains constant over the next 18 months. From a developmental point of view, the data lead us to interpret early walking (the first 5 months) as a process of integration of postural constraints into the dynamic necessities of gait movement. A second phase, beginning after 4 to 5 months of independent walking, is considered to be a tuning phase characterized by a more precise adjustment of the gait parameters.  相似文献   
77.
A motion equation in relative phase was developed that incorporates the spatial-temporal pattern of the bipedal gallop along with the more commonplace patterns of the bipedal jump and walk-run. In 3 experiments, human participants (N = 6 per experiment) simulated the bipedal gait patterns through the rhythmic motions of hand-held pendulums. Predictions of the motion equation for coordination equilibria and their respective degrees of stability were confirmed. In particular, the gallop pattern was less stable than the fundamental in-phase and antiphase patterns but changed in qualitatively similar ways to those gaits as a function of limb asymmetry and movement frequency. The relation between the modeled coordination dynamics and the kinematic characteristics of real bipedal galloping is discussed.  相似文献   
78.
79.
RESUMEN

Bronckart comienza revisando su trayectoria científica y explicando cuáles son sus dos enfoques básicos: el análisis del contexto o de situaciones y el análisis del lenguaje en función de sus operaciones subyacentes definidas en el marco de las teorías de la enunciación. Seguidamente justifica su renuncia a posiciones estructuralistas porque no tienen en cuenta el problema de las relaciones funcionales ni prestan atención al contexto. El autor relaciona las operaciones psicológicas con universales funcionales diferenciándolos de los estructurales de Chomsky y del enfoque de Searle, defendiendo su interés por la lingüística comparativa. Seguidamente aborda el tema de análisis de textos en ambientes escolares, sugiriendo que hay que sensibilizar a los educadores sobre el hecho de que constantemente estamos en situaciones de comunicación que van cambiando y crear instrumentos para que puedan analizar su propia práctica. Con relación al aspecto cognitivo del lenguaje, enfatiza la proliferación excesiva de los enfoques representativos y seguidamente desarrolla qué entiende por especificidad de la naturaleza del lenguaje y de su carácter comunicativo. Esta entrevista aborda, además, cuestiones tales como el fenómeno presuposicional, las aportaciones de Vygotsky o las experiencias de enseñar lenguaje a chimpancés, y finaliza planteando algunos aspectos de la teoría de Culioli.  相似文献   
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号