首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   41641篇
  免费   907篇
  国内免费   14篇
  2020年   251篇
  2019年   242篇
  2018年   3810篇
  2017年   3099篇
  2016年   2582篇
  2015年   427篇
  2014年   431篇
  2013年   1610篇
  2012年   1156篇
  2011年   3004篇
  2010年   2834篇
  2009年   1890篇
  2008年   2257篇
  2007年   2712篇
  2006年   566篇
  2005年   695篇
  2004年   626篇
  2003年   559篇
  2002年   470篇
  2001年   826篇
  2000年   844篇
  1999年   605篇
  1998年   258篇
  1997年   230篇
  1996年   202篇
  1992年   486篇
  1991年   424篇
  1990年   493篇
  1989年   407篇
  1988年   385篇
  1987年   367篇
  1986年   389篇
  1985年   443篇
  1984年   339篇
  1983年   287篇
  1982年   221篇
  1979年   324篇
  1978年   259篇
  1977年   206篇
  1976年   202篇
  1975年   302篇
  1974年   348篇
  1973年   353篇
  1972年   276篇
  1971年   243篇
  1970年   208篇
  1969年   230篇
  1968年   290篇
  1967年   251篇
  1966年   272篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
We examined the functional role of verbalizations in the generalization of self-instructional training with preschoolers. Children learned to overtly self-instruct during classroom work periods prior to covert training. Data were collected on children's acquisition of verbal regulation during training and on overt use of self-instructions in the classroom generalization setting. Results of a multiple baseline design across subjects indicated that treatment effects were evident in the training setting but did not generalize to the classroom until children were emitting overt self-instructions in the classroom itself. The production of self-verbalizations in the generalization setting was related to changes in correct responding, on-task behavior, and efficiency in completing academic work.  相似文献   
102.
Using a multi-trial, free-recall paradigm (Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; Lezak, 1983), the acquisition and retention by learning disabled (LD) young adults with a Halstead Impairment Index less than or equal to 0.3 (low LD) or greater than or equal to 0.4 (high LD) was compared with that of nondisabled and head-injured (HI) young adults. Significant group differences, particularly during the last three acquisition trials, confirmed the effectiveness of this method in revealing subtle learning and retention deficits. Analyses of Trial 1 recall showed group differences in the prerecency but not the recency portion of the serial position curve, thus implicating long-term but not short-term memory differences between the groups. Analyses of pair frequency (Sternberg & Tulving, 1977) data showed that the three clinical groups had difficulty organizing the to-be-learned material for effective retrieval. Following an interpolated task, retention was poorer for the clinical groups than for the nondisabled and varied inversely with the degree of neuropsychological impairment. Finally, during retention, the nondisabled and low LD groups retained and used the list organization established during acquisition whereas the high LD and HI groups did not.  相似文献   
103.
Tobacco, alcohol, and drug use are problems for American-Indian people. We reviewed these problems and the explanations for them and described a bicultural competence skills approach for preventing substance abuse with American-Indian adolescents. Data from a study of that approach suggest its efficacy with American-Indian youth. At posttest and a 6-month follow-up, American-Indian subjects who received preventive intervention based on bicultural competence skills concepts improved more than did American-Indian subjects in a no-intervention control condition on measures of substance-use knowledge, attitudes, and interactive skills, and on self-reported rates of tobacco, alcohol, and drug use. Our findings have implications for future substance-abuse prevention research with American-Indian people.  相似文献   
104.
In Korea, two number systems are in simultaneous use. One is somewhat irregular (like the English language number system), and the other regular (similar to the Chinese number system). This natural experiment allows a direct test of the hypothesis that structural regularity of the number system facilitates the acquisition of counting. Korean children initially suffer from their numerical ‘bilingualism’: they find it difficult to learn two counting systems at the same time. Yet, after this initial difficulty, they soon master the generative rules which are transparent in the regular system. The cognitive advantage provided by the regular number system then combines with other factors, like intensive teaching and parental pressure, to produce school achievement in mathematics superior to US children's.  相似文献   
105.
An Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
  相似文献   
106.
107.
108.
109.
110.
The startle probe response: a new measure of emotion?   总被引:17,自引:0,他引:17  
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号