全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53704篇 |
免费 | 922篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
54634篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 651篇 |
2019年 | 766篇 |
2018年 | 1041篇 |
2017年 | 1100篇 |
2016年 | 1140篇 |
2015年 | 819篇 |
2014年 | 975篇 |
2013年 | 4130篇 |
2012年 | 1758篇 |
2011年 | 1824篇 |
2010年 | 1148篇 |
2009年 | 1085篇 |
2008年 | 1540篇 |
2007年 | 1547篇 |
2006年 | 1356篇 |
2005年 | 1186篇 |
2004年 | 1149篇 |
2003年 | 1078篇 |
2002年 | 1086篇 |
2001年 | 1715篇 |
2000年 | 1667篇 |
1999年 | 1247篇 |
1998年 | 543篇 |
1997年 | 528篇 |
1996年 | 462篇 |
1995年 | 469篇 |
1993年 | 469篇 |
1992年 | 1018篇 |
1991年 | 910篇 |
1990年 | 992篇 |
1989年 | 869篇 |
1988年 | 903篇 |
1987年 | 837篇 |
1986年 | 879篇 |
1985年 | 798篇 |
1984年 | 703篇 |
1983年 | 660篇 |
1982年 | 461篇 |
1979年 | 718篇 |
1978年 | 543篇 |
1975年 | 616篇 |
1974年 | 709篇 |
1973年 | 687篇 |
1972年 | 581篇 |
1971年 | 530篇 |
1970年 | 484篇 |
1969年 | 579篇 |
1968年 | 650篇 |
1967年 | 570篇 |
1966年 | 512篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Autonomic determinism: the modes of autonomic control, the doctrine of autonomic space, and the laws of autonomic constraint. 总被引:17,自引:0,他引:17
Contemporary findings reveal that the multiple modes of autonomic control do not lie along a single continuum extending from parasympathetic to sympathetic dominance but rather distribute within a 2-dimensional space. The physiological origins and empirical documentation for the multiple modes of autonomic control are considered. Then a formal 2-dimensional conception of autonomic space is proposed, and a quantitative model for its translation into a functional output surface is derived. It is shown that this model (a) accounts for much of the error variance that has traditionally plagued psychophysiological studies, (b) subsumes psychophysiological principles such as the law of initial values, (c) gives rise to formal laws of autonomic constraint, and (d) has fundamental implications for the direction and interpretation of a wide array of psychophysiological studies. 相似文献
992.
J. L. A. Garcia 《Synthese》1991,86(3):349-360
This paper criticizes the thesis that intending to do something is reducible to some combination of beliefs and desires. Against Audi's recent formulation of such a view I offer as counterexample a case wherein an agent who wants and expects to V has not yet decided whether to V and hence does not yet intend to. I try to show that whereas belief that one will V is not necessary for intending to V, as illustrated in cases of desperate attempts to V, one cannot intend to V without preferring to V (rather than not V) and thus one cannot intend to V without, in some sense, wanting to V (at least wanting it in preference to not V-ing). The connection of one's intentions with one's objectives, attempts, plans, and hopes is briefly treated, and some influential work by Davidson is criticized. 相似文献
993.
994.
K C Klauer M Siemer J St?ber 《Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie》1991,38(3):379-393
We investigate the effects of emotional mood states on solving simple pattern comparison tasks. It is assumed that the effects of mood are mediated through response biases as well as effort expenditure. Departing from these assumptions, trend hypotheses are derived for the response biases evaluated by means of a signal detection analysis, for the response latencies, and for an adequate index of the underlying ability. The data support the hypotheses, and in particular, a positive mood state is found to improve performance. The assumption of mood-dependent differences in effort expenditure provides a parsimonious and integrative account of our results, while a reduced-capacity view cannot explain the findings obtained under positive mood induction. 相似文献
995.
H G Smid W Lamain M M Hogeboom G Mulder L J Mulder 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(3):696-714
This study was designed to test whether information transmission between the perceptual and motor levels occurs continuously or in discrete steps. Ss performed visual search across nontargets that shared visual features with one of two possible targets, each assigned to a different response. In addition to reaction time, psychophysiological measures were used to assess the duration of target search and the onset of central and peripheral motor activity. Nontargets sharing features with a target selectively activated the response associated with that target, even when it was not present in the display. This suggests that information transmission to the motor level can consist of fine-grained visual information and that visual search and response selection occur in parallel. 相似文献
996.
Task dynamics and resource dynamics in the assembly of a coordinated rhythmic activity. 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
G P Bingham R C Schmidt M T Turvey L D Rosenblum 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):359-381
Task dynamics corresponding to rhythmic movements emerge from interactions among dynamical resources composed of the musculature, the link segments, and the nervous and circulatory systems. This article investigated whether perturbations of interlimb coordination might be effect over circulatory and nervous elements. Stiffness of wrist-pendulums oscillated at a common tempo and at 180 degrees relative phase was perturbed through the use of tonic activity about an ankle. Left and right stiffnesses, the common period, and the phase relation all changed. Stiffnesses increased with ankle torque in proportion to the wrist's inertial load. Despite different changes in stiffness at the two wrists, isochrony was preserved. The stability was shown to be consistent with the proportionality of changes in stiffness to the inertial loads. The phase departed from antiphase in proportion to the asymmetry of inertial loads. The size of departures decreased with increasing ankle torque. An account was developed in terms of muscular, circulatory, and nervous functions. 相似文献
997.
K Kilborn 《Brain and language》1991,41(2):275-288
The traditional clinical picture for English nonfluent aphasics has generally presented the deficit as one of total loss of control over grammatical morphology, with some sparing of word order. This is at odds with recent research involving nonfluent aphasic speakers of highly inflected languages, which has shown that agrammatic performance is characterized by morphological substitution rather than omission errors. If the deficit associated with focal brain damage cannot be adequately accounted for in syndrome-specific ways, we may need to look for language-specific processing explanations. One such explanation has to do with language-specific response to global processing difficulty. The current experiment is designed to study the effects of a stress-related limitation on morphological processing. Normal speakers of a language with a relatively rich morphological system (German) are compared with those of a comparatively impoverished system (English) on different forms of a sentence comprehension task. In one form, "clean" stimuli permit full reliance on all available cues to meaning in each language. In another test, a low-level noise mask partially obscured the stimulus sentences. English speakers, who rely almost exclusively on word order cues, were not affected by the noise manipulation. German speakers relied heavily on morphological and semantic information rather than on word order under "clean" conditions. However, under noise Germans made significantly less use of grammatical morphology, with a trend toward compensatory reliance on word order. The results indicate that a global reduction in processing capacity can affect some aspects of language more than others and suggest that such factors must be taken into account in trying to understand specific impairment of morphology in aphasia. 相似文献
998.
999.
Twenty reading comprehension-disabled (CD) and 20 reading comprehension and word recognition-disabled (CWRD), right-handed male children were matched with 20 normal-achieving age-matched controls and 20 normal-achieving reading level-matched controls and tested for left ear report on dichotic listening tasks using digits and consonant-vowel combinations (CVs). Left ear report for CVs and digits did not correlate for any of the groups. Both reading-disabled groups showed lower left ear report on digits. On CVs the CD group showed a high left ear report but only when there were no priming precursors, such as directions to attend right first and to process digits first. Priming effects interfered with the processing of both digits and CVs. Theoretically, the CWRD group seems to be characterized by a depressed right hemisphere, whereas the CD group may have a more labile right hemisphere, perhaps tending to overengagement for CV tasks but vulnerable to situational precursors in the form of priming effects. Implications extend to (1) subtyping practices in research with the learning-disabled, (2) inferences drawn from studies using different dichotic stimuli, and (3) the neuropsychology of reading disorders. 相似文献
1000.
L Hochhaus K M Marohn 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):422-432
A visual prime succeeded by a brief target produces a paradox. Namely, target repetition yields poorer identification accuracy and shorter duration judgments than unrelated prime-target pairs. Experiment 1 manipulated stimulus onset asynchrony to learn when repetition blindness is maximized. Experiments 2 and 3 manipulated expectancy of repetitions through changes in the proportion of repeated trials and instructions, respectively. Results indicate that repetition blindness is influenced by subject strategies and that the change is not mediated by response bias. Experiment 4 showed that increasing the spatial distance between prime and target reduced but did not eliminate repetition blindness. The current data support joint explanation of repetition blindness in terms of perceptual capture (prime-target fusion) and token individuation failure (problems in encoding episodic reoccurrences of an event). 相似文献