首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   59891篇
  免费   2196篇
  国内免费   27篇
  2020年   623篇
  2019年   750篇
  2018年   970篇
  2017年   1069篇
  2016年   1130篇
  2015年   820篇
  2014年   970篇
  2013年   4535篇
  2012年   1791篇
  2011年   1812篇
  2010年   1159篇
  2009年   1127篇
  2008年   1646篇
  2007年   1592篇
  2006年   1420篇
  2005年   1309篇
  2004年   1232篇
  2003年   1186篇
  2002年   1215篇
  2001年   1734篇
  2000年   1670篇
  1999年   1262篇
  1998年   598篇
  1997年   586篇
  1992年   1125篇
  1991年   1024篇
  1990年   1092篇
  1989年   992篇
  1988年   983篇
  1987年   954篇
  1986年   996篇
  1985年   967篇
  1984年   867篇
  1983年   805篇
  1982年   587篇
  1981年   612篇
  1979年   946篇
  1978年   681篇
  1977年   599篇
  1976年   570篇
  1975年   815篇
  1974年   905篇
  1973年   940篇
  1972年   756篇
  1971年   748篇
  1970年   691篇
  1969年   752篇
  1968年   894篇
  1967年   837篇
  1966年   759篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
62.
63.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
Three experiments examined the relationships among plan complexity, access to planned actions, and verbal fluency while pursuing a persuasion goal. Consistent with theoretical expectations, complex planners were less fluent than less complex planners under high access conditions. Persons whose access was raised were less fluent than those whose access was not raised. A second experiment showed that reduced fluency was not induced by lowering of self-confidence. The third experiment replicated the findings of the second experiment and demonstrated that the questioning procedure used to raise access in the first two experiments produced increases in plan complexity as suggested by theory. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for the study of strategic communication.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号