全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53243篇 |
免费 | 644篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
53889篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 325篇 |
2019年 | 454篇 |
2018年 | 3864篇 |
2017年 | 3232篇 |
2016年 | 2738篇 |
2015年 | 620篇 |
2014年 | 616篇 |
2013年 | 2485篇 |
2012年 | 1516篇 |
2011年 | 3301篇 |
2010年 | 3006篇 |
2009年 | 2030篇 |
2008年 | 2534篇 |
2007年 | 2939篇 |
2006年 | 859篇 |
2005年 | 951篇 |
2004年 | 896篇 |
2003年 | 773篇 |
2002年 | 757篇 |
2001年 | 1049篇 |
2000年 | 984篇 |
1999年 | 731篇 |
1998年 | 408篇 |
1997年 | 331篇 |
1996年 | 323篇 |
1992年 | 551篇 |
1991年 | 547篇 |
1990年 | 533篇 |
1989年 | 543篇 |
1988年 | 536篇 |
1987年 | 499篇 |
1986年 | 523篇 |
1985年 | 572篇 |
1984年 | 457篇 |
1983年 | 435篇 |
1982年 | 359篇 |
1981年 | 343篇 |
1979年 | 523篇 |
1978年 | 379篇 |
1976年 | 362篇 |
1975年 | 435篇 |
1974年 | 487篇 |
1973年 | 508篇 |
1972年 | 409篇 |
1971年 | 383篇 |
1970年 | 381篇 |
1969年 | 409篇 |
1968年 | 489篇 |
1967年 | 422篇 |
1966年 | 423篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
801.
Antonucci M Lancioni GE O'Reilly MF Singh NN Sigafoos J Oliva D 《Perceptual and motor skills》2006,103(1):83-88
This study evaluated the extent to which two microswitches used as interfaces would enable a man with multiple disabilities and limited motor behavior to operate an electric door opener at the entrance of the day center that he attended. Analysis showed the man (a) learned to use the two microswitches, (b) preferred the microswitch placed at his wheelchair's footrest (which also allowed more effective responding) over the microswitch placed at his wheelchair's armrest, and (c) maintained his positive performance at the 1- and 2-mo. postintervention checks. The results were discussed from a technical and practical standpoint and in terms of implications for the quality of life of persons with disabilities. 相似文献
802.
Flynn FJ Reagans RE Amanatullah ET Ames DR 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2006,91(6):1123-1137
The authors argue that high self-monitors may be more sensitive to the status implications of social exchange and more effective in managing their exchange relations to elicit conferrals of status than low self-monitors. In a series of studies, they found that high self-monitors were more accurate in perceiving the status dynamics involved both in a set of fictitious exchange relations and in real relationships involving other members of their social group. Further, high self-monitors elevated their social status among their peers by establishing a reputation as a generous exchange partner. Specifically, they were more likely than low self-monitors to be sought out for help and to refrain from asking others for help. This behavior provides one explanation for why high self-monitors acquire elevated status among their peers--they are more attuned to status dynamics in exchange relations and adapt their behavior in ways that elicit status. 相似文献
803.
Theory and research dealing with self-regulation have focused primarily on instances of self-regulation that involve high levels of self-reflection and effortful self-control. However, intentionally trying to control one's behavior sometimes reduces the likelihood of achieving one's goals. This article examines the process of hypo-egoic self-regulation in which people relinquish deliberate, conscious control over their own behavior so that they will respond more naturally, spontaneously, or automatically. An examination of spontaneously occurring hypo-egoic states (such as flow, deindividuation, and transcendence) suggests that hypo-egoic states are characterized by lowered self-awareness and/or an increase in concrete and present-focused self-thoughts. In light of this, people may intentionally foster hypo-egoism via two pathways-(a) taking steps to reduce the proportion of time that they are self-aware (such as repeating a behavior until it is automatic or practicing meditation) or (b) increasing the concreteness of their self-thoughts (such as inducing a concrete mindset or practicing mindfulness). In this way, people may deliberately choose to regulate hypo-egoically when effortful control might be detrimental to their performance. 相似文献
804.
Rosemary E. Vile Junod Asha K. Jitendra Robert J. Volpe Kristi S. Cleary 《Journal of School Psychology》2006,44(2):87-104
The current study examined the behaviors related to academic engagement exhibited by students with ADHD during instruction in math and reading. A total of 155 students (92 ADHD, 63 recruited controls) in grades 1 through 4 participated in the study. Results revealed that students with ADHD exhibited statistically significant lower rates of academic engagement and higher rates of off-task behaviors than recruited controls and randomly selected peer comparisons. Specifically, students with ADHD exhibited statistically significant lower rates of passive academic engagement (PET) than recruited controls and peer comparisons; however no group differences were found with regard to active engaged time (AET). In addition, students with ADHD exhibited lower rates of AET than PET, a finding observed among both recruited controls and randomly selected peer comparisons. Implications for research and practice are discussed. 相似文献
805.
Facial transplantation, although controversial, is proposed as a major advance in facial reconstructive surgery, with the first partial transplant having taken place in France in November 2005. Although the psychological impact of facial transplantation will not be understood until several procedures have been carried out, this article examines the psychological issues likely to arise with particular reference to body image. A detailed framework for anticipation and management of psychological change is proposed. Pre-operative preparation must include thorough psychological preparation for the patient and their family. The immediate post-operative period is likely to be challenging, and a detailed management plan is proposed emphasising early return to function; subsequent psychological issues including altered body image, anxiety, shame, depression, communication and behavioural avoidance are discussed and a management strategy based on cognitive behavioural principles is proposed for the first post-operative year. Previous discussion frames psychological outcome in terms of complication and risk, tending to downplay the potential advantages of a successful procedure; the focus of this paper is on ensuring psychological adjustment as an inevitable consequence of change. 相似文献
806.
Rasch analysis was used to illustrate the usefulness of item-level analyses for evaluating a common therapy outcome measure of general clinical distress, the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1994). Using complementary therapy research samples, the instrument's 5-point rating scale was found to exceed clients' ability to make reliable discriminations and could be improved by collapsing it into a 3-point version (combining scale points 1 with 2 and 3 with 4). This revision, in addition to removing 3 misfitting items, increased person separation from 4.90 to 5.07 and item separation from 7.76 to 8.52 (resulting in alphas of .96 and .99, respectively). Some SCL-90-R subscales had low internal consistency reliabilities; SCL-90-R items can be used to define one factor of general clinical distress that is generally stable across both samples, with two small residual factors. 相似文献
807.
148 young adolescents who reported low frustration tolerance also reported more trait anger, state anger, and hostility. 相似文献
808.
Fernandez S Malcarne VL Malacrne VL Wilfley DE McQuaid J 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2006,12(3):403-419
The factor structure of the Bulimia Test--Revised (BULIT-R) was investigated using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory factor analysis (EFA). The sample consisted of 2,671 female college students (African American, Asian American, Caucasian American, and Latino American). Reliability coefficients were excellent across groups. African Americans scored significantly lower on the BULIT-R than Caucasian Americans. Across groups, CFA and EFA results suggest a six-factor solution is most appropriate. Consistent across groups were factors representing bingeing, body image, purging, and extreme weight loss behaviors, while few differences were observed across groups. These findings suggest that the measure is reliable and valid for use with diverse ethnic groups. Future research should focus on culturally salient psychological correlates of disordered eating in diverse ethnic groups. 相似文献
809.
810.
We explored college students' discrimination of complex visual stimuli that involvedmultiple-item displays. The items in each of the displays could be all the same, all different, or diverse mixtures of some same and some different items. The participants had to learn which of two arbitrary responses was correct for each of the displays without being told about the sameness or differentness of the stimuli. We observed a general improvement in discrimination performance--a rise in choice accuracy and a fall in reaction time-as the number of icons in the display was increased, even when the participants had been trained from the outset with displays containing different numbers of items and when smaller numbers of items were not randomly distributed but grouped in the center of the display. The participants' discrimination behavior also depended on the mixture of same and different items in the displays. Striking individual differences in the participants' discrimination behavior disclosed that people sometimes respond as do pigeons and baboons trained with a similar task. This and previous related research suggest that variability discrimination may lie at the root of same-different categorization behavior. 相似文献