全文获取类型
收费全文 | 75793篇 |
免费 | 1347篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
77145篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 855篇 |
2019年 | 977篇 |
2018年 | 1374篇 |
2017年 | 1439篇 |
2016年 | 1505篇 |
2015年 | 1057篇 |
2014年 | 1291篇 |
2013年 | 6177篇 |
2012年 | 2399篇 |
2011年 | 2456篇 |
2010年 | 1500篇 |
2009年 | 1499篇 |
2008年 | 2108篇 |
2007年 | 2050篇 |
2006年 | 1894篇 |
2005年 | 1594篇 |
2004年 | 1640篇 |
2003年 | 1548篇 |
2002年 | 1526篇 |
2001年 | 2434篇 |
2000年 | 2297篇 |
1999年 | 1757篇 |
1998年 | 835篇 |
1997年 | 706篇 |
1996年 | 763篇 |
1994年 | 702篇 |
1993年 | 692篇 |
1992年 | 1427篇 |
1991年 | 1319篇 |
1990年 | 1333篇 |
1989年 | 1259篇 |
1988年 | 1226篇 |
1987年 | 1173篇 |
1986年 | 1175篇 |
1985年 | 1245篇 |
1984年 | 990篇 |
1983年 | 874篇 |
1982年 | 684篇 |
1979年 | 1068篇 |
1978年 | 807篇 |
1975年 | 903篇 |
1974年 | 1000篇 |
1973年 | 1010篇 |
1972年 | 864篇 |
1971年 | 812篇 |
1970年 | 710篇 |
1969年 | 755篇 |
1968年 | 905篇 |
1967年 | 803篇 |
1966年 | 780篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
941.
Abstract— Generating a use far a visual word in comparison with reading the word aloud activates frontal attention areas first (170 MS), a left lateral frontal area next (250 ms), and then a left temporoparietal (Wernicke's) area (650 ms). A brief period of practice reduces these activations. If subjects are asked to respond to a word from the same practiced list by giving a novel use, the original activations reappear and are joined by activity similar in location and time to Wernicke's activation but in the right hemisphere. These findings demonstrate the time course of activations of neuroanatomical areas in word processing and indicate a role for the right hemisphere when semantic processing is more difficult, such as in generating a less frequent association in the presence of a highly practiced one. 相似文献
942.
Annelies E.M. Van Vianen Marcus G. Prins 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》1997,5(2):101-114
The development of attitudes and activities of newcomers in the 18 months following the first period of socialization was investigated. At two points in time, persons' preferences for seven work climates were compared with perceived climate. Changes in person-climate fit were assessed, using a squared difference index, after alternative fit-indices were tested with hierarchical regression analysis. Person-climate fit and work attitudes at T1 (eight to ten months after entry) were examined by comparing three groups of respondents who did not change jobs, changed jobs within the organization or changed organizations. Results showed no alpha, beta or gamma changes for the total group of 108 respondents. Alpha changes were only found for respondents who changed jobs. They showed higher ratings for job satisfaction at T2. Finally, work attitudes at T1 were related to turnover. Organizational changers showed higher turnover intentions and higher discrepancies between preferred and perceived risk orientation and work pressure at T1. 相似文献
943.
Abstract— Procrastination is variously described as harmful, innocuous, or even benefical. Two longitudinal studies examined procrastination among students. Procrastinators reported lower stress and less illness than nonprocrastinators early in the semester, but they reported higher stress and more illness late in the term, and overall they were sicker. Procrastination thus appears to be a self-defeating behavior pattern marked by short-term benefits and long-term costs. 相似文献
944.
945.
Iser G. Deleon Brian A. Iwata Han-Leong GOH April S. Worsdell 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1997,30(3):439-449
Tustin (1994) recently observed that an individual's preference for one of two concurrently available reinforcers under low schedule requirements (concurrent fixed-ratio [FR] 1) switched to the other reinforcer when the schedule requirements were high (concurrent FR 10). We extended this line of research by examining preference for similar and dissimilar reinforcers (i.e., those affecting the same sensory modality and those affecting different sensory modalities). Two individuals with developmental disabilities were exposed to an arrangement in which pressing two different panels produced two different reinforcers according to progressively increasing, concurrent-ratio schedules. When two dissimilar stimuli were concurrently available (food and a leisure item), no clear preference for one item over the other was observed, regardless of the FR schedules in effect (FR 1, 2, 5, 10, and 20). By contrast, when two similar stimuli were concurrently available (two food items), a clear preference for one item emerged as the schedule requirements were increased from FR 1 to FR 5 or FR 10. These results are discussed in terms of implications for conducting preference assessments and for selecting reinforcers to be used under training conditions in which response requirements are relatively high or effortful. 相似文献
946.
947.
948.
949.
950.
We evaluated strategies to increase behaviors associated with courteous provision of service by 3 staff members of a human service agency. Training included written instructions, practice, and performance feedback. A lottery procedure was introduced to maintain courteous service after training. The results of a multiple baseline design across the 3 participants showed marked increases in courteous behaviors following training. These effects were maintained at 3-, 5-, and 8-month follow-ups. Consumers' satisfaction with service also increased. These findings suggest that simple training and reinforcement procedures can enhance courtesy afforded those who receive service from public and nonprofit organizations. 相似文献