首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   411篇
  免费   20篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   10篇
  2021年   12篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   33篇
  2017年   19篇
  2016年   12篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   15篇
  2013年   51篇
  2012年   34篇
  2011年   20篇
  2010年   28篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   13篇
  2005年   21篇
  2004年   14篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   10篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   1篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1986年   1篇
排序方式: 共有431条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
401.
Qualitative differences between three types of media used for the laboratory induction of emotion (film clips, still images, and music) were explored using a clinical sample. The relative effectiveness at inducing negative emotion and the degree to which the induction elicited personally relevant experiences of emotion was examined. Nomothetic and idiographic media were included as stimuli, and comparisons were made across media types and selection type. Nomothetic and idiographic media did not significantly differ in levels of negative affect endorsed. Significant differences did emerge between media and selection types in degree of personal relevance, with nomothetic images rated lowest and idiographic music rated highest. Significant differences also emerged in ratings of positive affect, with greater personal relevance significantly related to greater positive affect. The qualitative differences in emotion induction procedures found in this study are discussed in relation to the potential importance of matching emotion induction procedures to specific emotion-related processes of interest when used for the laboratory study of emotion and psychopathology.  相似文献   
402.
Historically, physicians have expressed concern about their patients’ risk of genetic discrimination, which has acted as a barrier to uptake of genetic services. The Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act of 2008 (GINA) is intended to protect patients against employer and health insurance discrimination. Physicians’ awareness and knowledge of GINA has yet to be evaluated. In 2009, we mailed surveys to 1500 randomly selected members of the American Academy of Family Physicians. Questions measured physicians’ current knowledge of GINA and their level of concern for genetic discrimination. In total, 401 physicians completed the survey (response rate 26.9%). Approximately half (54.5%) of physicians had no awareness of GINA. Of physicians who reported basic knowledge of GINA, the majority were aware of the protections offered for group health insurance (92.7%), private health insurance (82.9%), and employment (70.7%). Fewer physicians were aware of GINA’s limitations regarding life insurance (53.7%) and long-term care insurance (58.8%). Physicians demonstrated highest levels of concern for health insurance, life insurance, and long-term care insurance discrimination, with less concern for employer and family/social discrimination. Level of concern for the risk of genetic discrimination did not correlate significantly with awareness of GINA. Approximately 17 months after GINA was signed into federal law, physicians’ knowledge remained limited regarding the existence of this legislation and relevant details. Physicians who are aware of GINA continue to have significant concerns regarding the risk of genetic discrimination. This study reveals the need to further educate physicians about the existence of GINA and the protections offered.  相似文献   
403.
404.
Early life stress (ELS) experiences can cause changes in cognitive and affective functioning. This study examined the persistent effects of a single traumatic event in infancy on several adult behavioral outcomes in male and female C57BL/6J mice. Mice received 15 footshocks in infancy and were tested for stress-enhanced fear learning, extinction learning, discrimination and reversal learning, and novel object recognition. Infant trauma potentiated fear learning in adulthood and produced resistance to extinction but did not influence other behaviors, suggesting restricted effects of infant trauma on behaviors reliant on cortico-amygdala circuitry.

Exposure to traumatic events early in childhood is associated with the development of psychiatric disorders (Copeland et al. 2018) and deficits in cognitive and affective functioning (Pechtel and Pizzagalli 2011) in adulthood. In humans, early life stress (ELS) is defined as experiencing traumatic events in childhood (Pechtel and Pizzagalli 2011). Rodent models of ELS suggest that acute versus chronic stress may differentially alter systems that regulate the stress response, producing different behavioral outcomes in adulthood (Pryce et al. 2002; Musazzi et al. 2017).Stress-enhanced fear learning (SEFL), a powerful preclinical model of PTSD- and addiction-like behaviors, captures the enduring, maladaptive effects of a single traumatic event on behavior (Rau et al. 2009; Meyer et al. 2013; Radke et al. 2019). In these studies, exposure to 15 footshocks enhances contextual fear conditioning later in life. For infant SEFL, enhanced contextual fear conditioning occurs months after the initial stressful experience and in the absence of memory for the context in which ELS was experienced (Poulos et al. 2014; Quinn et al. 2014). SEFL protocols have been used to model adult or infant trauma in rats (e.g., Rau et al. 2009; Poulos et al. 2014; Quinn et al. 2014) and have been extended to adult mice (Sillivan et al. 2017; Hassien et al. 2020; Pennington et al. 2020). However, to our knowledge, no studies have established the use of acute, infant footshock as a model of ELS in mice.We sought to characterize the effects of acute, infant trauma exposure across several types of learning in adult mice. We tested mice exposed to 15 footshocks on postnatal day (PND) 17 for contextual fear learning, extinction of fear, discrimination and reversal learning, and novel object recognition. Our results suggest that exposure to acute infant trauma enhances fear learning and resistance to extinction in adulthood, but does not alter other types of learning.Male and female C57BL/6J mice were generated from breeding pairs from The Jackson Laboratory. Mice were group-housed (two to four mice/cage) post-weaning and were provided food and water ad libitum, unless otherwise specified. Mice were on a 12:12 light/ dark cycle. Other than trauma exposure, all behavioral tests were conducted during adulthood (PND 60+). Animals were cared for in accordance with the guidelines set by the National Institutes of Health and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Miami University.We first established that exposure to infant footshock produces enhanced contextual fear conditioning in adulthood. Mice were placed in a MED-Associates conditioning chamber (context A) on PND 17 (Fig. 1A; after Quinn et al. 2014). Context A was brightly lit, contained a uniform grid floor, was scented with vanilla (50%), and was cleaned with odorless 5% sodium hydroxide. Mice received either 0 or 15 footshocks (1 mA, 1 sec) during a 60-min session beginning 180 sec following placement in the chamber. Progressive scan video cameras containing visible light filters monitored mice throughout the session. Video Freeze software (Med Associates, Inc.) analyzed the video and data were expressed as percent of time spent freezing during the session. Fear conditioning experiments were powered to detect sex differences with an n of eight per sex. Because differences were not observed when sex was included as a factor in analyses, all reported results represent data from both sexes. Due to computer malfunction, fear conditioning sessions from 14 mice were hand scored according to standard time-sampling procedures (Chowdhury et al. 2005) and data from three mice had to be excluded on extinction session 2.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Acute infant trauma produces stress-enhanced fear learning in adulthood. (A) Experimental timeline and visual representation of context A and context B. Infant trauma consisted of 15 footshocks or no footshocks on PND 17 in context A. Adult fear conditioning consisted of one footshock (black bars) or no footshock (blue bars) on PND 60 in context B. Extinction was assessed in context B. Memory of the infant trauma was assessed in context A. Stress enhanced fear learning (SEFL) was observed in mice who were exposed to early life stress (15 shocks) and fear conditioning (one shock) for extinction test 1 (B) and extinction test 2 (C). (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01 versus no/one shock group (Holm–Sidak test). (D) Mice showed little freezing, indicating an absence of fear memory for the infant trauma in context A. Data are means ± SEM.Contextual fear conditioning occurred on PND 60 in a novel context (context B). Context B was dark with a staggered grid floor and cleaned and scented with acetic acid (5%). Baseline freezing during the first 180 sec in this novel context was assessed and used to measure generalization between the stress exposure context (context A) and the novel fear conditioning context (context B). Mice received either 0 or 1 footshocks (1 mA, 1 sec) 180 sec into a 3.5-min session. Thus, there were four groups (infant trauma/adult fear conditioning): no/no shock, n = 14; 15/no shock, n = 15; no/one shock, n = 15; and 15/one shock, n = 18. To test extinction of fear memory, mice were reintroduced to context B for two 8-min sessions, separated by 24 h. Finally, mice were reintroduced to context A for an 8-min retention test of the original context.Mice exposed to 15 footshocks in infancy and conditioned with one footshock as adults exhibited robust SEFL. There were no differences in baseline freezing during the adult fear conditioning session (infant trauma: F(1,47) = 2.67, P = 0.109, no/no shocks = 0.26 ± 0.15; no/one shock = 2.17 ± 0.64; 15/no shocks = 2.14 ± 0.76; 15/one shock = 7.17 ± 3.25; all data mean ± SEM). For extinction sessions (Fig. 1B,C), a mixed-effects analysis identified main effects of trauma (F(1,58) = 5.68, P = 0.020), fear conditioning (F(1,58) = 12.40, P < 0.001), and session (F(1,55) = 5.25, P = 0.026). There was a significant interaction between fear conditioning and session (F(1,55) = 7.10, P = 0.010). Two-way ANOVA was next used to examine each test session. For session 1 (Fig. 1B), there were main effects of trauma (F(1,58) = 5.72, P = 0.020) and fear conditioning (F(1,58) = 21.10, P < 0.001). The interaction of trauma and fear conditioning did not reach the threshold for significance (F(1,58) = 2.87, P = 0.096). Follow-up Holm Sidak''s tests revealed that adult fear conditioning produced greater freezing in mice exposed to infant footshock vs. trauma-naïve mice (P = 0.008). For the second extinction test (Fig. 1C), the main effects of trauma (F(1,55) = 3.72, P = 0.059) and fear conditioning (F(1,55) = 3.92, P = 0.053) approached the threshold for significance. Follow-up Holm Sidak''s tests revealed that trauma-exposed mice froze more than trauma-naïve mice following adult fear conditioning only (P = 0.042). When tested for memory of the context used for trauma-exposure on PND 17 (context A), mice demonstrated minimal freezing and two-way ANOVA found no significant main effects or interactions (Fs < 1.39) (Fig. 1D). Percent freezing in infancy and in extinction test 1 in adulthood were correlated (r = −0.481, P = 0.043) for the 15/one group alone. Activity bursts in infancy did not correlate with freezing behavior in adulthood. These results indicate that mice exposed to infant trauma who experienced fear conditioning in adulthood exhibited SEFL on the first and second extinction sessions but the memory of the trauma experience was not retained into adulthood.Since PTSD is associated with deficits in fear extinction (Zuj et al. 2016), we next examined extinction learning using 30-min sessions (Fig. 2A). Mice were exposed to acute infant trauma (0 or 15 footshocks) on PND 17. On PND 60, mice were reintroduced to context A for an 8-min test of memory for the original ELS context prior to fear conditioning (no/one shock = 2.41 ± 0.58; 15/one shock = 3.50 ± 0.42; no/three shocks = 1.86 ± 0.43). On PND 61 fear conditioning occurred in context B. Since we observed that trauma-naïve mice displayed very little fear conditioning following one footshock (Fig. 1B,C), mice in this experiment received either one or three footshocks during adult fear conditioning. There were three groups (infant trauma/adult fear conditioning): no/one shock, n = 17; 15/one shock, n = 17; no/three shocks, n = 18. Retention of fear memory was tested for five subsequent days (PND 62–66) in context B during 30-min sessions.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Extinction of fear learning is impaired following acute infant trauma. (A) Experimental timeline. Infant trauma consisted of 15 footshocks or no footshocks on PND 17 in context A. Adult fear conditioning consisted of one (yellow symbols) or three (blue symbols) footshocks in trauma-naïve mice and one footshock (black symbols) in trauma-exposed mice in context B. Memory of the infant trauma was assessed in context A. Mice received five extinction sessions in context B. (B) On extinction day 1, trauma-exposed mice initially froze at the same level as trauma-naïve mice conditioned with three footshocks but fear persisted longer, demonstrating within-session resistance to extinction. (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01 15/one shock group versus no/one shock group; (#) P < 0.05 15/one shock group versus no/three shocks group; () P < 0.05, () P < 0.01 no/one shock group versus no/three shock group (Holm–Sidak test). (C) Freezing was averaged across the first 8 min of each extinction session. Trauma-exposed mice demonstrated resistance to extinction across sessions. On session 1, both trauma-exposed (15/one shock group) and mice conditioned with three footshocks (no/three shock group) had elevated freezing compared to mice conditioned with one footshock. On session 2, freezing in trauma-exposed mice remained elevated and was greater than in the other two groups. (**) P < 0.01 15/one shock group versus no/one shock group. (##) P < 0.01 15/one shock group versus no/three shocks group. () P < 0.05 no/one shock group versus no/three shocks group (Holm–Sidak test). Data are means ± SEM.On the first extinction session, there were significant main effects of time (F(29,1421) = 2.23, P < 0.001) and group (F(2,49) = 3.54, P = 0.037) and a significant interaction (F(58,1421) = 1.93, P < 0.001). Holm–Sidak follow-up comparisons revealed that trauma-exposed mice (15/one shock) and mice conditioned with three footshocks during adulthood (no/three shocks) froze more than the no/one shock group during the first 3 min (P < 0.05 for minute 1 and P < 0.01 for minutes 2 and 3) of the first extinction session (Fig. 2B). Trauma-exposed mice continued to freeze more than the no/one shock group during minutes 4–7 (P < 0.01) and minute 11 (P < 0.05). Freezing in the no/3 shocks group was similar to trauma-exposed mice for the first 4 min but diminished sooner, evidenced by a significant difference between these groups during minutes 5, 6, and 9 (P < 0.05). These results suggest that conditioning with three footshocks produces similar levels of fear in trauma-naïve mice as conditioning with one footshock in trauma-exposed mice, but that within-session extinction is delayed in the trauma-exposed group.To examine extinction across the five sessions, we averaged freezing during the first 8 min of each session. We found significant main effects of session (F(4,196) = 22.92, P < 0.001) and group (F(2,49) = 6.38, P = 0.003) and a significant interaction (F(8,196) = 2.75, P = 0.007). Holm–Sidak follow-up comparisons revealed that mice exposed to infant trauma (15/one shock) and mice in the no/three shock group froze more than those in the no/one shock group on extinction session 1 (P < 0.01 and P < 0.05, respectively) (Fig. 2C). On session 2, freezing was greater in trauma-exposed mice versus both other groups (P < 0.01). Percent freezing in infancy did not correlate with freezing behavior in adulthood. Activity bursts in infancy correlated with freezing behavior in adulthood on extinction test 1 (r = −0.558, P = 0.020). These results further suggest that infant trauma produces resistance to extinction.To determine whether the behavioral alterations observed in mice exposed to infant trauma extend to other types of learning, we tested the effects of infant footshock on operant discrimination and reversal learning for food reward and novel object recognition. For discrimination and reversal learning, we used a subset of mice from the first experiment (no shock = 20, 15 shocks = 17) restricted to 85% of free-feeding weight. Mice underwent one habituation day with ten 14-mg grain pellets (Bio Serv) in their home cages. The following day, mice began 15-min training sessions in a standard mouse operant chamber (Med Associates). There were two nose-poke holes and a reward receptacle on one wall of the chamber and a house light and speaker on the opposite wall. The chamber was housed in a sound and light-attenuating box and connected to a computer for data collection (Med-PC V software suite). For all sessions, the house light was off and lights in the nose-poke holes were on. Following a correct response, a 2-sec, 65-dB tone sounded and there was a timeout period of 20 sec following reward delivery during which the lights above the nose-poke holes were off and no rewards could be earned.During the first session, 30 pellets were automatically delivered into the reward receptacle. Next, mice were trained to respond for the food reward on a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule by responding at either nose-poke hole until meeting criterion of 30 responses in 15-min. For discrimination, mice were trained to respond at the active nose-poke hole (100% probability of reward), which was randomly assigned to the left or right side. The contingencies of the active and inactive nose-pokes holes were reversed once criterion was met (≥30 rewards with 85% reinforced responses over two consecutive sessions).Neither sex nor adult fear conditioning affected any measure of discrimination and reversal learning, so all results are reported collapsed across these two factors. Two trauma-exposed females did not acquire the discrimination in 25 sessions and were not advanced to reversal. Data were analyzed using mixed-effects analyses with phase (i.e., acquisition and reversal) as the within-subjects factor. There were no differences between groups in the total number of sessions required to complete discrimination (no shock = 5.70 ± 0.69; 15 shock = 6.82 ± 1.50) or reversal (no shock = 9.40 ± 0.98; 15 shock = 7.73 ± 1.21). Mice made more total reinforced (main effect of phase: F(1,33) = 11.62, P = 0.002) and total nonreinforced (main effect of phase: F(1,33) = 42.54, P < 0.001) responses during reversal but there were no effects of infant trauma on behavior (Fig. 3A,B). These results indicate that acute infant trauma does not influence acquisition of operant discrimination learning or behavioral flexibility in adulthood.Open in a separate windowFigure 3.Acute infant trauma does not affect discrimination and reversal learning or novel object recognition. (A,B) Following infant trauma on PND 17 (no footshock, blue, or 15 footshocks, black), adult mice were trained to respond for a food pellet in an operant, spatial discrimination and reversal learning task. Following acquisition of the discrimination (left or right nose-poke hole reinforced 100% of the time) the contingencies of the responses were reversed. Mice made more total reinforced (A) and unreinforced (B) responses during reversal versus acquisition across sessions ([**] P < 0.01, main effect of training phase) but there were no effects of infant trauma exposure. (C) A separate cohort of mice exposed to infant trauma (no or 15 footshocks on PND 17) was tested for novel object recognition by assessing exploration of a novel object 1 and 24 h after exposure to the familiar object. Mice explored the novel object more ([**] P < 0.01, main effect of object, main effect of testing session, and interaction of object × testing session) but there were no effects of infant trauma exposure. Data are means ± SEM.In a new cohort of mice (no shock = 16, 15 shocks = 16), novel object recognition following infant footshock was tested. In adulthood, mice were handled for 1–3 min for two consecutive days. The following day, mice were placed in 20 × 18 × 25-cm apparatus with white floors and patterned walls (Panlab) for a 10-min habituation session to the chamber. Twenty-four hours later, mice were returned to the apparatus now containing two sample objects for 10-min initial exposure to the objects (two identical small plastic caps, 0.8 cm tall with a 2.1-cm diameter) placed in the back right and left corners of the apparatus. One hour later, mice were returned to the apparatus for a 3-min session (1-h test) where one of the sample objects was replaced with a novel object (a 2.5 × 1.2 × 1.5-cm Lego tower). The object replaced was alternated for each mouse. Mice were returned to the same box 24 h later for another 3-min session (24-h test) with the opposite cap replaced with a second novel object (a 3 × 1.5-cm black binder clip). ANY-maze software recorded each session. Time spent interacting with each object was measured by two independent raters and averaged (after Bevins and Besheer 2006).A three-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of object (familiar vs. novel; F(1,60) = 59.84, P < 0.001). There was also a main effect of testing session (1-h vs. 24-h test; F(1,60) = 7.89, P = 0.007) and an interaction of object × testing session (F(1,60) = 11.48, P = 0.001) (Fig. 3C,D). Interrater reliability was confirmed using Pearson''s correlation (r = 0.942). These results indicate that acute infant trauma does not influence hippocampal-dependent object recognition memory.Our results demonstrate that an acute traumatic experience during infancy affects some learned behaviors during adulthood. As previously reported in rats (Quinn et al. 2014; Poulos et al. 2014), 15 footshocks on PND 17 increased adult contextual fear conditioning in mice. We also demonstrated for the first time that the infant SEFL protocol produces resistance to extinction (within-session and between-session) and that behavior in a discrimination and reversal learning task and a novel object recognition task are unaffected. These findings establish the use of infant footshock to study SEFL in mice and further support the use of this paradigm as a model of PTSD-like behavior.The effects observed here differ from those commonly observed following chronic ELS manipulations such as limited nesting and bedding (Ivy et al. 2008; Molet et al. 2016) or maternal separation (Nishi et al. 2014). Chronic ELS impairs acquisition of fear conditioning in adult rodents (Kosten et al. 2006; Stevenson et al. 2009; Lesuis et al. 2019) and performance on hippocampal-dependent memory tasks (Rice et al. 2008; Naninck et al. 2015), for example. These behavioral differences suggest that acute and chronic infant stressors alter neural circuits in unique ways that are worthy of further study.Since the tasks used here rely on distinct neural circuits, the current results provide novel insight into how acute infant trauma impacts brain function. The effects of acute ELS were restricted to fear acquisition and extinction, suggesting alterations in cortico-amygdala circuits (Tovote et al. 2015). However, preservation of novel object recognition as well as discrimination and reversal learning suggest that hippocampal and striatal circuits likely remain intact (Cohen and Stackman 2015; Izquierdo et al. 2017). These findings can guide future studies concerning the neural mechanisms of acute ELS effects on behavior.  相似文献   
405.
The use of instructional advance directives assumes that preferences for life-sustaining medical treatment remain stable over time and across changes in life condition. A sample of 332 older adults recorded their preferences for 4 life-sustaining treatments in 9 illness scenarios. These preferences were elicited again 1 and 2 years after the original interview. Overall, preferences for life-sustaining treatment were moderately stable over time, but stability varied significantly across judgments. Preferences were most stable for illness scenarios that were most and least serious and for decisions to refuse treatment. Age, gender, education, and prior completion of an advance directive were all related to preference stability, and evidence indicated that declines in physical or psychological functioning resulted in decreased interest in life-sustaining treatment.  相似文献   
406.
407.
The current study examined the association between central adiposity, measured by waist circumference, and cardiovascular reactivity to stress among 106 White and 105 Black adolescents, approximately 50% of whom were girls. Participants engaged in 4 laboratory tasks while cardiovascular reactivity measures were taken. Independent of body mass index, race, and gender, participants with a greater waist circumference exhibited greater systolic blood pressure reactivity and diastolic blood pressure reactivity (boys only). Race did not affect the results. Results from the present study suggest that central adiposity is associated with blood pressure reactivity early in life, especially in adolescent boys.  相似文献   
408.
409.
410.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号