首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1080篇
  免费   54篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   13篇
  2021年   23篇
  2020年   18篇
  2019年   17篇
  2018年   56篇
  2017年   42篇
  2016年   31篇
  2015年   27篇
  2014年   23篇
  2013年   131篇
  2012年   55篇
  2011年   56篇
  2010年   40篇
  2009年   30篇
  2008年   38篇
  2007年   35篇
  2006年   37篇
  2005年   41篇
  2004年   29篇
  2003年   39篇
  2002年   41篇
  2001年   29篇
  2000年   27篇
  1999年   16篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   5篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   14篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   5篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   6篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   5篇
  1969年   10篇
  1965年   5篇
排序方式: 共有1134条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
Saccadic reaction time (RT) is reduced when the fixation point is removed shortly before target onset. Although Tam and Stelmach (1993) argued that thisgap effect could not be explained solely by the idea that fixation offset disengaged visual attention and preferred an explanation based on disengagement of the oculomotor system, they felt that they could not rule out a hybrid model in which both oculomotorand attentional disengagement contribute to the gap effect. Our analysis of the dual response experiment (Experiment 4), upon which this hybrid model was based, shows that manual and saccadic responses were likely compromised by a grouping or delay strategy and that subjects may not have been attending as instructed. On these grounds, we argue that Tam and Stelmach (1993), like Kingstone and Klein (1990; 1993a) provide no evidence that attentional disengagement contributes to the gap effect. An alternative proposal (Klein & Kingstone, 1993), that motor preparation and oculomotor disengagement combine additively to produce the gap effect, is consistent with the data from Tam and Stelmach’s Experiments 1–3, is similar to the explanation that they prefer, and has been strongly supported when directly tested (Kingstone, Klein, & Taylor, 1994).  相似文献   
942.
The existence of a phonemic deficit that is predictive of, and probably causal to, many cases of reading difficulty is well established. Tallal (1984) has suggested that this phonemic deficit is in fact a symptom of an underlying auditory temporal processing deficit. Our purpose in this paper is to evaluate the plausibility of this hypothesis. The various components that might constitute sequential (or temporal) processing are described. Our review of the literature reveals considerable evidence for a deficit in dyslexics in stimulus individuation tasks (e.g., gap detection) and temporal order judgments in both the auditory and visual modalities. The possibility that a general temporal processing deficit is associated with dyslexia, as suggested by Tallal (1984), is explored, and possible etiologies for such a deficit are discussed. The possibility of a causal link between temporal processing deficits and some reading disabilities is demonstrated, and converging evidence from morphological studies is reviewed. It is concluded that a temporal processing deficit does appear to be present in many developmental dyslexics, and strategies are suggested for further research aimed at evaluating the hypothesis that this deficit may be the root cause of a number of cases of dyslexia itself.  相似文献   
943.
Unpredictive visual transient cues have a biphasic effect on reaction times (RTs) to peripheral onset targets. At relatively short (e.g., 150-msec) cue-target stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs), RTs to targets at cued versus uncued locations are facilitated, whereas at relatively long SOAs (e.g., beyond 300 msec), they are inhibited. The present review explores the conditions under which this latter, inhibitory, effect-referred to as inhibition of return (IOR; Posner & Cohen, 1984)—is revealed and those conditions under which it is generated. We argue that the extant literature converges on the view that IOR reflects a motor response bias that is generated by the activation of an oculomotor program to fixate the cue. However, we reveal that current conceptualizations of IOR are based on a limited sampling of possible tests of the generation and measurement of IOR and indicate where further experimental research is critical.  相似文献   
944.
945.
An Excel macro is presented for averaging spreadsheet data. The macro has several special features: (1) The data are weighted by the inverse variance of each datum to decrease the contribution-of noisy outliers. (2) There is a provision for a power or a log transform of the data before averaging. The rationale for transforming the data before averaging is discussed (3) The output includes the average value, its standard error, and the reduced chi-square that measures the goodness of fit (4) The standard error is corrected by a heterogeneity factor based on the reduced chi-square The averaging of data is rarely done properly, and the intent of this article is to clarify the issues and provide a tool that allows researchers to improve their averaging techniques.  相似文献   
946.
A review of published work over the past decade and a half supports the effectiveness of the large group in clinical settings. Greater sophistication among practitioners has led to combining administrative, psychotherapeutic, and sociotherapeutic large groups and to the use of effective treatment teams and clearly defined contracts in inpatient hospital work. Some research findings suggest that large treatment groups are as effective as small therapy groups with some patient populations. There appears to be a movement toward convergence in the techniques employed by individual, small group, and large group therapists in practice, while not in theory. This positive trend in the mental health field may lead to more flexible practitioners.He is also on the board of the A. K. Rice Institute and a faculty member of the Cincinnati Psychoanalytic Institute. He would like to thank Walter Stone and Peter Herr for their comments and Lorna Volk for editing.  相似文献   
947.
Treating the chronically mentally ill involves not only working with patients suffering from schizophrenia and other prolonged or recurrent psychotic illnesses, it also means providing treatment for patients with severe personality disorders. Many of these patients are also active substance abusers. This article examines the therapeutic and management issues raised for outpatient clinicians who work with these patients. Consideration is devoted to the special problems in treating the dual diagnosis patient, issues of patient and therapist safety, limit setting, splitting dynamics, and countertransference reactions. A set of recommendations is offered for conducting outpatient group therapy, specifying what is needed from both the clinicians and the facility in which this type of treatment is provided.  相似文献   
948.
In the preceding article it was argued that control theory is not a useful paradigm for understanding work motivation. In doing so, three primary criticisms were presented: (a) that control theory, as originally formulated, does not adequately describe human behavior, (b) that modifications of control theory to describe human behavior have not been successful, and (c) that the use of logical deductions to develop a control theory model of work motivation is less efficacious than a grounded theory approach. In this reply, it will be shown that (a) the first criticism is not incorrect but is an inappropriate basis for criticism, (b) statements regarding modified control theory models are either inaccurate or premature, and (c) both inductive and deductive reasoning play an important role in theory development. In addressing these issues and in discussing the distinct advantages of current control theory models it will be shown that control theory does provide a viable paradigm for understanding work motivation.Thanks to John R. Hollenbeck for his helpful comments on this paper.  相似文献   
949.
Klein W  Heuer H 《Acta psychologica》1999,103(3):311-329
We explored the structural constraints on concurrent movements and/or positions of the head and a steering device by means of a reaction-time task. In the first experiment, subjects had to respond rapidly to an imperative stimulus by way of rotating a handle-bar to the left or to the right and back to the central position while they maintained a left or right eccentric position of the head. Latency of the handle-bar responses did not depend on whether their initial directions were toward the eccentric head position or in the opposite direction, but kinematic characteristics did: iso-directional movements were of larger amplitude and longer duration until peak excursion. In the second experiment, the imperative stimuli for handle-bar rotations were presented at variable intervals after the head had been moved from the central to one of the eccentric positions and before its predictable return movement. Kinematic characteristics of the handle-bar rotations depended on the left and right eccentric head positions in the same way as in Experiment 1, but now iso-directional movements had a longer reaction time than movements in the direction of the forthcoming return movement of the head. These findings suggest that specifications of head-movement directions facilitate concurrent specifications of handle-bar rotations in the same direction and inhibit specifications of handle-bar rotations in the opposite direction, consistent with the notion of cross-talk during motor programming.  相似文献   
950.
Can psychiatric disorders be conceptualized as "harmful dysfunctions" (J. C. Wakefield, 1992a, 1992b, 1999; S. O. Lilienfeld & L. Marino, 1995)? Wakefield's (1992a) central concept of disorder as "harmful dysfunction" is discussed by placing it in the context of a complementary discussion of disease, illness, the sick role, and evolution (D. F. Klein, 1978). S. O. Lilienfeld and L. Marino (1995) contended that proper biological function cannot be determined. This argument obscures the key significance of involuntary impairment of evolved functions. The claim that the Roschian concept has no counterpart in reality is incorrect and does not support the conclusion that dysfunctions are irreducibly evaluative and therefore arbitrary. J. C. Wakefield's (1999) views in this area are supplemented. The role of monothetic and polythetic categorization, extremal terms, and the concept of normality in nosology is considered. This analysis refutes the implication that deviance and illness are equivalent. The resolution of this debate is practically relevant to emphasizing areas of research investment, such as therapeutics.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号