全文获取类型
收费全文 | 217篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
229篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 10篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 27篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 16篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1930年 | 1篇 |
1921年 | 1篇 |
1920年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有229条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Philosophical Studies - Logical realism is the view that there is logical structure in the world. I argue that, if logical realism is true, then we are deeply ignorant of that logical structure:... 相似文献
42.
Criminal activity often involves considerable risks. It is therefore not surprising that criminals have been speculated to
differ from noncriminals in risk attitude. Yet, few data exist to support this assumption. Moreover, the psychological underpinnings
of differences in risk attitude are currently little understood. We presented prisoners and controls with sets of risky decision
tasks and modeled their responses using cumulative prospect theory (CPT). The two groups showed several differences. Prisoners
were more risk seeking than nonprisoners in lotteries involving losses, but they were less risk seeking in lotteries involving
high-probability gains. Bestfitting CPT parameters indicated a reduced sensitivity to outcomes, for both gains and losses,
and a stronger loss aversion among prisoners. In addition, prisoners showed a diminished sensitivity to the probability of
gains. Our results contribute to a better understanding of prisoners’ risk attitudes and the underlying mechanisms that distinguish
prisoners from nonprisoners and may thus help improve interventions designed to prevent crime. 相似文献
43.
The current study investigated whether viewing X-rated movies, Internet access in the home, and gender of the participant would differ between age of first initiation for oral sex, age of first initiation for sexual intercourse, and number of sexual partners. An online sample of 437 participants with an average age of 29.46 participated in the study. Each participant completed a survey that assessed early sexual behaviors and Internet and X-rated material exposure. Results discovered males with Internet access during the ages of 12 to 17 reported significantly younger ages for first oral sex compared to males without Internet access. In addition, male and female participants with Internet access, between the ages 12 to 17, reported younger ages for first sexual intercourse compared to participants without Internet access. Study limitations and implications are discussed. 相似文献
44.
In 3 studies, consumers received information about products that had either positive or negative features in common. When they evaluated the products shortly afterward, the common features had little influence on their evaluations, and this was true regardless of whether they had made a preference rating, a similarity rating, or no rating at all before reporting these evaluations. In all cases, consumers apparently used one of the brands spontaneously as a standard of comparison for the other and, in doing so, discounted their common features. When their evaluation was delayed, however, shared features had greater impact. Two factors appear to be operating in the latter conditions. First, the alternative brand is less accessible and, therefore, is less likely to be used as a standard of comparison. Second, shared features are easier to recall than unique features, so they have a disproportional influence for this reason. 相似文献
45.
Manfred Kraus 《Argumentation》2007,21(1):3-19
In Roman rhetoric, contrarium was variably considered either a figure of speech or an argument. The paper examines the logical pattern of this type of
argument, which according to Cicero is based on a third Stoic indemonstrable syllogism: The persuasiveness of this type of argument, however, vitally depends on the validity of the alleged ‹incompatibility’ forming
its major premiss. Yet this appears to be the argument’s weak point, as the ‹incompatibilities’ employed generally hold for
the most part only, and are reducible to topical argument schemes. This is why in practical usage such arguments are most
often phrased as rhetorical questions, the persuasive force of which, enhanced by certain strategical maneuverings and fallacies,
makes the audience swallow the argument. 相似文献
46.
The impact of a modified dialectical behavior therapy and problem-solving model is examined on the work environment of a patient
with an obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD). A case study is presented examining the impact this client's OCPD
behavior in the work environment and on his subordinates. Discussed are interventions that were useful in treating a motivated
patient employing cognitive-behavioral models of treatment for this disorder. The case study focuses on the work setting and
the issues tied to the work environment wherein subordinates are required to work under a supervisor with OCPD. Lessons learned,
issues and import for further study are offered. 相似文献
47.
48.
Katharina Theresa Halicki Caterina Suitner Tobias Vogel Michaela Wnke 《European journal of social psychology》2021,51(1):166-177
The mental imagination of (social) actions has been shown to follow a left‐to‐right trajectory, with the thematic agent associated with the left position (Spatial Agency Bias, Suitner & Maass in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 2016, p. 245–301). For example, individuals asked to choose a picture that visualizes the sentence “Tom kicks George” tend to choose an image where the agent, Tom, is positioned on the left‐hand side rather than on the right‐hand side. However, as an alternative to the thematic role of the agent, such findings may reflect a mental representation following pragmatic relevance. Specifically, a pragmatic perspective holds that word order and syntactic functions are strategic devices to communicate that the element is important for the sentence. Thus, positioning in the described picture‐matching task may actually reflect the agent's pragmatic relevance instead of agency per se. As a test, we vary whether sentences are written in the active versus the passive voice. Results from five studies indicate that the passive voice results in the tendency to place the agent on the right‐hand side rather than on the left‐hand side of a picture. Instead, the acted‐upon person is positioned on the left‐hand side of a picture. A sixth experiment reveals that for the passive voice, the agent is still seen as more agentic than the receiver, but is considered less relevant. The findings are congruent with the proposed pragmatic relevance account. Implications for the Spatial Agency Bias as well as for building mental representations in general are discussed. 相似文献
49.
50.
The possible connections between social variables such as frequency of social activity and subjective loneliness on the one hand, and extent of mass media use on the other, have received relatively little empirical attention. In this investigation, two related hypotheses concerning such links are proposed. The compensation hypothesis holds that persons with little social contact or greater loneliness will be more likely to utilize mass media to compensate for social impoverishment. The buffering hypothesis more specifically predicts that such compensation is effective; that is, persons “at risk” for loneliness because of diminished social contact may be at least partially protected from subjective loneliness due to heavy use of mass media. The hypotheses were tested in two samples: college students and a group of adolescent males attending a summer camp. Little support was found for the compensation hypothesis. While limited support was found for the buffering hypothesis, analyses also revealed an unexpected opposite effect; for the music variable, heavy media use actually intensified the association between social deficits and subjective loneliness. Some limitations of this study are discussed, and issues related to the unexpected “intensification” effects are presented. 相似文献