全文获取类型
收费全文 | 950篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
959篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 24篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 21篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 108篇 |
2012年 | 35篇 |
2011年 | 36篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 37篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 28篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 16篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 18篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1935年 | 9篇 |
1931年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有959条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
We investigated personality disorders (PDs) of young men with chronic authority conflicts using the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-I (MCMI-I; Millon, 1983). PDs occurred considerably more often in the young men with chronic authority conflicts (62%) compared with a clinical control group of young men with acute authority conflicts (39%). The prevalence of PDs in a normal control group was considerably lower (11%). The passive-aggressive PD is over-represented among young men with chronic authority conflicts; there is hardly any antisocial PD. Millon's conceptualization of these disorders could have influenced these results. The high prevalence of passive-aggressive PDs complicates a good psychotherapeutic outcome in young men with chronic authority conflicts. Our results underscore the clinical utility of the controversial PAPD diagnosis. 相似文献
102.
Self-awareness and identity are important cornerstones of thinking in social psychology. On the basis of current knowledge and theories from the social, cultural, and biological sciences, this paper attempts to outline an integrative approach to the phenomena of memory and reminiscence. Reference to psychodynamic argumentation is made in addition, where appropriate. The central topic to our argumentation is autobiographical memory, which we analyse according to evolutionary, neuroscientific, and cultural findings. The emotional context and the ontogenetic development of reminiscences (or memories directly relating to one's own self) provide the preliminary framework for an integrated view which includes interactions between the life span, brain development, the social and cultural environment, and genetic predispositions. 相似文献
103.
104.
105.
Shifting attention is an effortful control process and incurs a cost on the cognitive system. Previous research suggests that rewards, such as monetary gains, will selectively enhance the ability to shift attention when this demand for control is explicitly cued. Here, we hypothesized that prospective monetary gains will selectively enhance the ability to shift attention even when control demand is unpredictable and not cued beforehand in a modality shift paradigm. In two experiments we found that target detection was indeed facilitated by reward signals when an unpredictable shift of attention was required. In these crossmodal trials the target stimulus was preceded by an unpredictive stimulus directing attention to the opposite modality (i.e., visual–auditory or auditory–visual). Importantly, there was no reward effect in ipsimodal trials (i.e., visual–visual or auditory–auditory). Furthermore, the absence of the latter effect could not be explained in terms of physical limits in speed of responding. Potential motivation of monetary rewards thus selectively translates into motivational intensity when control (i.e., switching) is demanded in unpredictable ways. 相似文献
106.
Hans Lenk 《Axiomathes》2018,28(6):619-637
New accents in the philosophy of technology and philosophy of science amounting, e.g., to the so-called schools of the “New Experimentalism”, “New Instrumentalism” and, recently, “New Mechanism” emphasize the impact of instruments, experiments, and “mechanisms” of the respective technologies opened up by the progress of ever-improving measuring instruments, procedures etc. In addition, it would be necessary to accentuate the process- and action-orientation including practical responsibility problems and dynamic systems models from an epistemological perspective of the methodological scheme-interpretationist approach developed by the author since the seventies of last century. The necessity of multi-disciplinary mutual influences was already time and again stressed regarding philosophy of science. But relatively new is not only the methodological turn involved but also the necessity to go to higher levels of abstraction, metatheories, action- and value-orientation. Therefore, we have to go more interdisciplinary, systems-oriented, and higher-level. That implies notable consequences for an ethics, or rather, meta-ethics for philosophy of science. Practice-oriented philosophers of (techno-)science will have to go more methodological, multidisciplinary and even (meta-)responsible. 相似文献
107.
108.
Lesions of the basal ganglia, thalamus, and deep white matter: Differential effects on language functions 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Claus-W. Wallesch Hans H. Kornhuber Richard J. Brunner Thomas Kunz Beate Hollerbach Gebhard Suger 《Brain and language》1983,20(2):286-304
Forty-five patients with unilateral demarcated vascular lesions in the basal ganglia, the thalamus and the deep white matter were investigated with an "aphasia battery." Patients with basal ganglia lesions performed worse than both other groups in tests of articulation, syntax, and lexical functions. The deficit of patients with basal ganglia lesions on all expressive language modalities was lateralized to the left hemisphere. Patients with left thalamic lesions showed impairments of speech fluency and in the Token Test. Patients with white matter lesions alone showed no effect of laterality in tests of language functions. The results are discussed on the basis of a recent theory of the participation of the deep nuclei in language processing. 相似文献
109.
110.