首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   703篇
  免费   27篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   5篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   16篇
  2019年   13篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   25篇
  2016年   29篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   90篇
  2012年   40篇
  2011年   49篇
  2010年   21篇
  2009年   29篇
  2008年   46篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   26篇
  2005年   30篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   20篇
  2002年   14篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   15篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   9篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   6篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   4篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   2篇
  1967年   2篇
排序方式: 共有731条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Previous research on the relationship between self‐monitoring and job performance has neglected the role of tenure even though both theory and preliminary evidences suggest that job incumbents' tenure should be a strong moderator. The current study investigates the role of organizational tenure on the relationship between self‐monitoring and job performance (number of new contracts sold) in a sample of sales insurance agents. Consistent with expectations, tenure moderates the relationship between self‐monitoring and job performance, i.e., self‐monitoring is positively correlated with job performance for job incumbents with less tenure but not for those with more tenure. We propose that the relationship between self‐monitoring and job performance depends on the job incumbents' ability and opportunity to control the cues available to those who interact with and/or appraise the performance of the job incumbent.  相似文献   
72.
73.
74.
75.
This article presents a comprehensive conceptualization of the structure of worry and the relation of worry to mental health and well-being. It is assumed that worries have two facets, namely, the object of a worry (e.g., self, close others, society, the world) and the domain of a worry (the field of life with which it is concerned). The object of a worry is presumed to be more important than its domain in determining the impact of worries on mental health. Only worries concerned with self and close others (micro worries) are expected to be related to poor mental health, whereas worries about society or the entire world (macro worries) are expected to be positively related to mental health. An instrument is introduced to study worries in accordance with the proposed structure. Its validity is tested by two confirmatory techniques (similarity structure analysis and confirmatory factor analysis) in samples from Israel, West Germany, and (the former) East Germany. Worry scores are also related to five mental health indicators. Results show that across samples micro and macro worries can validly be distinguished. Micro worries are strongly related to poor mental health, whereas macro worries are unrelated to mental health or relate marginally to positive well-being.  相似文献   
76.
Can Reliabilists Believe in Subjective Probability?   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
According to reliabilist conceptions of knowledge, knowledge implies reliable true belief. Since reliability is an irreducibly probabilistic notion, one's view of knowledge also depends on one's view of probability. If one believes that all probability is subjective probability, knowledge becomes a relativized concept: knowledge is relative to a given body of beliefs of a given person at a given time. Since such a relativized conception of knowledge is extremely implausible and since reliabilism seems to capture at least part of the truth, one should rather give up a purely subjective view of probability.  相似文献   
77.
Studies with the Experience Sampling Method (ESM) have shown that individuals experience more flow at work than at leisure. This indicates that people enjoy working but paradoxically at the same time leisure activities are preferred (“paradox of work”). We took a second look at the paradox of work by measuring flow directly, including affect measures, and differentiating between active and passive leisure activities. We also adopted a dynamical approach based on the idea that the transitions of experiences has to be taken into account to get the total picture. For the period of 1 week, data of 100 employees with different professions were collected with the ESM (in total 4,504 measurements). In accordance with existing work, we found that flow was high during work. It was slightly (albeit significantly) higher than during active leisure activities and considerably higher than during passive leisure activities. At the same time, negative activation was low during passive and active leisure activities and lower than during work and vice versa for valence (happiness). Thus, leisure activities do have a positive affective quality that could explain why people prefer leisure to work. Regarding the dynamical approach, we tested whether flow during work would lead to higher valence in leisure but could not support this assumption. We discussed the findings and made suggestions for future research. In an additional analysis, we reveal that perceived outcome experience could partially explain why work and leisure experience differed.  相似文献   
78.
79.
Attentional bias to gain- and loss-related stimuli was investigated in a dot-probe task. We used coloured stimuli that had acquired their valence during the experiment by signalling the chance to either win or lose points in a game task. Replicating previous findings with the additional singleton paradigm, we found attentional bias effects for both gain- and loss-related colours. The effects were due to delayed disengagement from valent stimuli, especially if they were positive, and could not be explained by nonattentional processes like behavioural freezing. Our findings suggest that stimuli signalling opportunities and dangers hold attention, supporting a general motivational relevance principle of the orienting of attention.  相似文献   
80.
A total of 19 forensic patients with the diagnosis of a personality disorder participated in a schema-oriented psychotherapy (SOPT) over a period of approximately 2 years. Treatment was carried out in three groups which were separated according to diagnoses from clusters B and C of personality disorders in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders text revision (DSM-IV-TR). The control group of 19 forensic patients received treatment as usual and were paralleled for relevant forensic variables. The SOPT was implemented to raise patient awareness for their maladapted schemas and to modify the associated dysfunctional coping strategies. On the basis of differentiated individual case concepts, therapeutic interventions were carried out at cognitive and behavioral levels. Furthermore, activating therapeutic methods were implemented during treatment sessions to actualize maladapted schemas on an emotional level and to enhance awareness. Treatment effects were assessed by standardized self-rating, staff rating and objective measures. The results showed more positive changes for the schema-oriented treatment groups in comparison to the control group. In contrast staff rating for patients in the control group showed a clear deterioration of the clinical features.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号