全文获取类型
收费全文 | 334篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
352篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 31篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 14篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有352条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
A scale assessing generalized false self‐perceptions (Perceptions of False Self, POFS) was developed and tested across three studies involving a total of 331 adolescents (11–16 years). In Study 1, interviews were conducted to develop items for the scale. In Study 2, psychometric techniques were used to derive a scale composed of 16 items. Study 2 also assessed the validity of the scale: depressive symptoms were found to increase POFS across 10 weeks, whereas false self‐perceptions had a reciprocal effect on anxiety. In Study 3, the convergent validity of the POFS scale was established. Overall, the evidence suggests that the POFS scale is a reliable and valid measure of generalized false self‐perceptions. 相似文献
82.
J. Michael Dunn 《Studia Logica》1979,38(2):149-169
Given classical (2 valued) structures
and
and a homomorphism h of
onto
, it is shown how to construct a (non-degenerate) 3-valued counterpart
of
. Classical sentences that are true in
are non-false in
. Applications to number theory and type theory (with axiom of infinity) produce finite 3-valued models in which all classically true sentences of these theories are non-false. Connections to relevant logic give absolute consistency proofs for versions of these theories formulated in relevant logic (the proof for number theory was obtained earlier by R. K. Meyer and suggested the present abstract development). 相似文献
83.
84.
The relationship between individual differences in conjugate lateral eye movements (CLEMs) and inferential reasoning was investigated in two experiments. Subjects were given inference tasks in sentence and pictorial form and were later tested for recognition of correct inferences as well as literal recognition of the acquisition items. In Experiment 2, for example, subjects were given logical syllogisms during acquisition. Theoretically, right-eye movement/left-hemispheric (REM/LH) subjects should have excelled at this highly logical task but no significant differences between eye-movement groups were found. However, support for previous research showing the moderating effects of gender on hemispheric activation (measured by CLEMs) and hemispheric competence came from the memory data of Experiment 2. These data indicated that male REM/LH subjects recognized more acquisition sentences than the other eye movement/gender groups. It was suggested CLEMs and other measures of hemispheric activation and competence, coupled with data collected using complex cognitive tasks, can eventually lead to explanatory brain models of cognitive processing. 相似文献
85.
86.
Carla Rice Eliza Chandler Jen Rinaldi Nadine Changfoot Kirsty Liddiard Roxanne Mykitiuk Ingrid Mündel 《希帕蒂亚:女权主义哲学杂志》2017,32(2):213-229
This article explores twelve short narrative films created by women and trans people living with disabilities and embodied differences. Produced through Project Re?Vision, these micro documentaries uncover the cultures and temporalities of bodies of difference by foregrounding themes of multiple histories: body, disability, maternal, medical, and/or scientific histories; and divergent futurities: contradictory, surprising, unpredictable, opaque, and/or generative futures. We engage with Alison Kafer's call to theorize disability futurity by wrestling with the ways in which “the future” is normatively deployed in the service of able‐bodiedness and able‐mindedness (Kafer 2013 ), a deployment used to render bodies of difference as sites of “no future” (Edelman 2004 ). By re‐storying embodied difference, the storytellers illuminate ongoing processes of remaking their bodily selves in ways that respond to the past and provide possibilities for different futures; these orientations may be configured as “dis‐topias” based not on progress, but on new pathways for living, uncovered not through evoking the familiar imaginaries of curing, eliminating, or overcoming disability, but through incorporating experiences of embodied difference into time. These temporalities gesture toward new kinds of futures, giving us glimpses of ways of cripping time, of cripping ways of being/becoming in time, and of radically re‐presencing disability in futurity. 相似文献
87.
88.
89.
Nikolas Pautz;Kirsty McDougall;Katrin Mueller-Johnson;Francis Nolan;Alice Paver;Harriet M. J. Smith; 《Applied cognitive psychology》2024,38(1):e4162
Experiment-based voice parades often result in low hit-rates and high false-alarm rates. One contributing factor may be that the experimental procedures omit elements that might naturally occur in the memory formation process, such as the process of reflection. In Experiment 1 (N = 180, F = 92) we explored if a post-encoding reflection manipulation, compared to a simple attention control task, prior to a five-minute retention interval would improve identification performance. In Experiment 2 (N = 180, F = 93), we explored how the effects of this manipulation might change when the retention interval was 24-h. The results show that the inclusion of a reflection manipulation did not meaningfully improve performance in either experiment. Importantly, we found no meaningful difference in performance when directly comparing the two retention interval durations. We consider theoretical explanations for these results and discuss implications for the design and validity of earwitness voice parade studies. 相似文献
90.
Margaret A. McDevitt Roger M. Dunn Marcia L. Spetch Elliot A. Ludvig 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2016,105(1):23-40
Pigeons and other animals sometimes deviate from optimal choice behavior when given informative signals for delayed outcomes. For example, when pigeons are given a choice between an alternative that always leads to food after a delay and an alternative that leads to food only half of the time after a delay, preference changes dramatically depending on whether the stimuli during the delays are correlated with (signal) the outcomes or not. With signaled outcomes, pigeons show a much greater preference for the suboptimal alternative than with unsignaled outcomes. Key variables and research findings related to this phenomenon are reviewed, including the effects of durations of the choice and delay periods, probability of reinforcement, and gaps in the signal. We interpret the available evidence as reflecting a preference induced by signals for good news in a context of uncertainty. Other explanations are briefly summarized and compared. 相似文献