全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6700篇 |
免费 | 342篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
7043篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 76篇 |
2022年 | 68篇 |
2021年 | 124篇 |
2020年 | 146篇 |
2019年 | 209篇 |
2018年 | 257篇 |
2017年 | 261篇 |
2016年 | 299篇 |
2015年 | 211篇 |
2014年 | 218篇 |
2013年 | 788篇 |
2012年 | 379篇 |
2011年 | 401篇 |
2010年 | 228篇 |
2009年 | 228篇 |
2008年 | 331篇 |
2007年 | 277篇 |
2006年 | 227篇 |
2005年 | 228篇 |
2004年 | 234篇 |
2003年 | 234篇 |
2002年 | 172篇 |
2001年 | 119篇 |
2000年 | 133篇 |
1999年 | 90篇 |
1998年 | 69篇 |
1997年 | 54篇 |
1996年 | 47篇 |
1995年 | 41篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 35篇 |
1992年 | 58篇 |
1991年 | 51篇 |
1990年 | 75篇 |
1989年 | 60篇 |
1988年 | 61篇 |
1987年 | 39篇 |
1986年 | 50篇 |
1985年 | 46篇 |
1984年 | 42篇 |
1983年 | 37篇 |
1982年 | 25篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 27篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1972年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有7043条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
62.
R Loo 《Perceptual and motor skills》1978,46(2):633-634
The report noted that the use of high item-remainder coefficients as the sole criterion in the selection of items for short state-anxiety scales from full scales can lead to inaccurate interpretation of results. It was demonstrated that the use of a four-item state-anxiety scale from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory A-State scale based on high item-remainder coefficients changed relationships in a previously reported study which used full-scale scores. It was proposed that such changes in relationships were the result of selecting four items which happen to load on only one of three dimensions of the A-State scale. The criterion of high item-remainder coefficents alone may be inadequate in the formation of short state-anxiety scales. 相似文献
63.
This paper describes a computational method for weighted euclidean distance scaling which combines aspects of an analytic solution with an approach using loss functions. We justify this new method by giving a simplified treatment of the algebraic properties of a transformed version of the weighted distance model. The new algorithm is much faster than INDSCAL yet less arbitrary than other analytic procedures. The procedure, which we call SUMSCAL (subjectivemetricscaling), gives essentially the same solutions as INDSCAL for two moderate-size data sets tested.Comments by J. Douglas Carroll and J. B. Kruskal have been very helpful in preparing this paper. 相似文献
64.
Otto P. van Driel 《Psychometrika》1978,43(2):225-243
In the applications of maximum likelihood factor analysis the occurrence of boundary minima instead of proper minima is no exception at all. In the past the causes of such improper solutions could not be detected. This was impossible because the matrices containing the parameters of the factor analysis model were kept positive definite. By dropping these constraints, it becomes possible to distinguish between the different causes of improper solutions. In this paper some of the most important causes are discussed and illustrated by means of artificial and empirical data.The author is indebted to H. J. Prins for stimulating and encouraging discussions. 相似文献
65.
66.
Paul M.G. Emmelkamp Mieke van der Helm Berendien L. van Zanten Ivo Plochg 《Behaviour research and therapy》1980,18(1):61-66
Fifteen obsessive-compulsive patients were given treatment consisting of ten sessions of gradual exposure in vivo. For half of the patients gradual exposure in vivo was preceded by self-instructional training.Treatment resulted in significant improvement on anxiety and avoidance scales, Leyton Obsessional Inventory, Self-rating Depression Scale and on ratings for anxious mood and depression. Neither the post-test nor the follow-ups 1 month and 6 months later indicated a difference between the effects of the two conditions. Self-instructional training did not enhance the effectiveness of gradual exposure in vivo. 相似文献
67.
Richard A. van den Pol Brian A. Iwata Martin T. Ivancic Terry J. Page Nancy A. Neef F. Paul Whitley 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1981,14(1):61-69
This study examined classroom-based instruction in restauranting skills for handicapped persons. Three male students were taught each of four skill components in sequential order: locating, ordering, paying, and eating and exiting. Training was implemented in a multiple baseline design across subjects and consisted of modeling and role playing in conjunction with photo slide sequences and a simulated ordering counter. The use of a menu containing general item classes and a finger matching procedure for identifying errors in the delivery of change greatly reduced the reading and math skills necessary to enter and complete the program. Periodic probes were conducted in a McDonald's restaurant prior to, during, and up to one-year following the termination of training. In addition, two probes (overt and covert observation) were conducted in a Burger King restaurant to assess further generalization to a location different from the one depicted throughout training. Results showed that students' performance on restaurant probes improved as a result of training, generalized to novel settings, maintained over an extended period of time, and was comparable to that of a normative sample of nonretarded persons. 相似文献
68.
Comparison of the magnitude and intrinsic spread of foveal diplopia thresholds with the accuracy of ocular alignment as determined with a subjective alignment method shows that: the accuracy of alignment in the vertical direction (within 1–2 min) is remarkably good and much better than in the horizontal direction; the largest disparities occurring due to restricted alignment accuracy are usually substantially smaller than the foveal diplopia thresholds; interindividual variability in the magnitude of foveal diplopia thresholds is not due only to interindividual variability in the alignment accuracy; and the spread of foveal diplopia thresholds exceeds the spread of ocular alignment, which implies that the noise in the foveal disparity domain is not only due to the restricted alignment accuracy but also to sensory processes. Finally, the data confirm that, unlike the case with diplopia thresholds, the spread of stereoscopic thresholds is not affected by the restricted alignment accuracy. 相似文献
69.
We distinguish three different readings of the intuitionistic notions of validity, soundness, and completeness with respect to the quantification occurring in the notion of validity, and we establish certain relations between the different readings. For each of the meta-logical notions considered we suggest that the most natural reading (which is not the same for all cases) is precisely the one which is required by the recent intuitionistic completeness theorems for IPC.This paper was partly written while the first author, H. C. M. de Swart, was a Visiting Fellow at Princeton University for the academic year 1976–1977. This opportunity was given to him by the Niels Stensen Stichting in Amsterdam, the department of philosophy of the Catholic University in Nijmegen and by Princeton University. He is happy to express his appreciation for that opportunity. 相似文献
70.
Joop van der Pllgt Peter Ester Joop van der Linden 《European journal of social psychology》1983,13(4):437-439
Studied the effects of attitude extremity on perceived consensus and willingness to ascribe trait terms to others with either pro or anti nuclear attitudes. Results showed that attitude extremity affected consensus estimates. Trait attributions revealed a clear effect for valence, especially for the extreme attitude groups. Subjects with extreme attitudes also ascribed more traits to both pro and anti others than subjects with relatively moderate attitudes. 相似文献