首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4145篇
  免费   263篇
  2023年   31篇
  2022年   53篇
  2021年   67篇
  2020年   135篇
  2019年   104篇
  2018年   203篇
  2017年   197篇
  2016年   217篇
  2015年   168篇
  2014年   184篇
  2013年   620篇
  2012年   220篇
  2011年   226篇
  2010年   144篇
  2009年   149篇
  2008年   208篇
  2007年   200篇
  2006年   154篇
  2005年   102篇
  2004年   105篇
  2003年   120篇
  2002年   97篇
  2001年   74篇
  2000年   91篇
  1999年   46篇
  1998年   36篇
  1997年   30篇
  1996年   29篇
  1995年   16篇
  1994年   18篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   17篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   9篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   17篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   13篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   12篇
  1977年   9篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   9篇
  1972年   11篇
  1971年   14篇
  1970年   12篇
排序方式: 共有4408条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
891.
A simplified proof of a lemma by Ledermann [1938], which lies at the core of the factor indeterminacy issue, is presented. It leads to a representation of an orthogonal matrixT, relating equivalent factor solutions, which is different from Ledermann's [1938] and Guttman's [1955].T is used to evaluate bounds on the average correlation between equivalent sets of uncorrelated factors. It is found that the minimum average correlation is independent of the data.This paper owes much to an unknown reviewer.  相似文献   
892.
893.
It is assumed that the investigator has set up a simple structure hypothesis in the sense that he has specified the zero loadings of the factor matrix. The maximum-likelihood method is used to estimate the factor matrix and the factor correlation matrix directly without the use of rotation methods, and the likelihood-ratio technique is used to test the simple structure hypothesis. Numerical examples are presented.The work was supported by a grant (NSF-GB 1985) from the National Science Foundation to Educational Testing Service. Reproduction in whole or in part for any purpose of the United States Government is permitted.The work was carried out when the author was Visiting Research Statistician at Educational Testing Service. The author wishes to thank Dr. Frederic M. Lord for many helpful suggestions throughout the course of this study.  相似文献   
894.
895.
Ohne ZusammenfassungDie Literatur zur Logik in der Sowjetunion ist sehr bescheiden. Nebst einigen Kapiteln in grösseren Werken sind es folgende Schriften: Bochénski, I. M., Soviet Logic,SST, 1, 29–38. — Dahm, H., Renaissance der formalen Logik,Ost-Probleme, 1957,8, 254–267. — Philipov, A.,Logic and Dialectic in the Soviet Union, New York 1952, XI+89 pp. — Winkelmann, A., Die Stellung der formalen Logik im Sowjet-system,Scholastik, 1956,1, 85–89.  相似文献   
896.
897.
Developmental changes in intra- and interlingual interference were followed in 74 German-Swedish bilingual and 15 trilingual subjects using Stroop tasks for vision. The results provide evidence that the differential pattern of interference is mainly determined by language usage. Intra- and interlingual interference follow language dominance patterns in an everchanging process; hence, it is the point of measurement that determines whether equivalent or different amounts of interference are obtained. Stimuli characteristics determine points of language balance but are—like cognitive capabilities—of minor importance for the differential pattern of interference. Stroop task performance cannot be taken as evidence for or against the interdependence hypothesis of bilingual storage, since the Stroop situation does not permit one language to be turned off. Prolonged response times in multilinguals, found in this study, are more congruent with the interdependence hypothesis. By providing this developmental perspective, the current findings can explain previous contradictory results.This research was supported by the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation. It was reported in a paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Psycholinguistics and Foreign Language Learning, Åbo, Finland, October 25–6, 1982.  相似文献   
898.
The purpose of the present study was an attempt to describe clinical symptoms in terms of physiological reactions and negative cognitions experienced by phobic patients. A total of 267 clinical phobic patients divided among blood phobia, small animals phobia, dental phobia, claustrophobia, social phobia and agoraphobia, participated in the study. The patients within each phobic category were separated into a Conditioned and an Indirect Acquisition group, based on their answers to a set of specific questions concerning the etiology of the phobia. All patients rated (0–4) how intensively they experienced 11 specified physiological reactions and 10 negative cognitions when confronted with their phobic situation in vivo. A mean score was calculated for each item within each group, and an item rank-order hierarchy was set up for each group. Differences between items within each hierarchy were statistically evaluated by one-way ANOVAs and Turkey's HSD-tests. Results showed particular idiosyncratic patterns of responding for the blood phobic, agoraphobic and social phobic groups as well as several commonalities among all the groups concerning activation of specific physiological reactions and negative cognitions. A more fine-grained analysis of the data is best made by direct visual inspection of the figures in this paper.  相似文献   
899.
900.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号