全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1257篇 |
免费 | 44篇 |
专业分类
1301篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 42篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 25篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 112篇 |
2012年 | 43篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 40篇 |
2008年 | 52篇 |
2007年 | 40篇 |
2006年 | 33篇 |
2005年 | 32篇 |
2004年 | 38篇 |
2003年 | 31篇 |
2002年 | 27篇 |
2001年 | 35篇 |
2000年 | 49篇 |
1999年 | 36篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 13篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1967年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 17篇 |
1965年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1301条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
982.
983.
Billie M. Thompson Ph.D. Susan R. Andrews Ph.D. 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2000,35(3):174-188
This article provides an overview of the theoretical underpinnings of the Tomatis Method, along with a commentary on other
forms of sound/music training and the need for research. A public debate was sparked over the “Mozart Effect.” This debate
has turned out to be unfortunate because the real story is being missed.
The real story starts with Alfred Tomatis, M.D., scientist and innovator. Dr. Tomatis was the first to develop a technique
using modified music to stimulate the rich interconnections between the ear and the nervous system to integrate aspects of
human development and behavior. The originating theories behind the Tomatis Method are reviewed to describe the ear’s clear
connection to the brain and the nervous system. The “neuropsychology of sound training” describes how and what the Tomatis
Method effects.
Since Dr. Tomatis opened this field in the mid 20th century, no fewer than a dozen offshoot and related systems of training
have been developed. Though each new system of treatment makes clains of effectiveness, no research exists to substantiate
their claims. Rather, each simplified system bases its “right to exist and advertise” on the claimed relationship to Tomatis
and his complex Method. Research is desperately needed in this area.
The 50 years of clinical experience and anecdotal evidence amassed by Tomatis show that sound stimulation can provide a valuable
remediation and developmental training tool for people of all ages. Offshoot systems have watered down the Tomatis Method
without research to guide the decisions of simplifying the techniques and equipment. 相似文献
984.
985.
Experienced counsellors recorded clients' metaphors, their responses to the metaphors and their views on the effects of the interchange. Two of the three responses in Strong's model of counsellor response to clients' metaphors were used frequently. They were: explicating what is implicit in a metaphor and therapeutically extending or modifying it. The third response proposed by Strong'the counsellor creating and delivering a 'therapeutic metaphor''was used only once. A further kind of response was found: the counsellor recognizing and remembering a client's metaphor for possible future use. Overall, Strong's model was supported. The effect of counsellor orientation on counsellor response to clients' metaphors and the value and use of metaphors in counselling are briefly discussed, and some guidelines suggested. 相似文献
986.
Nairne JS Thompson SR Pandeirada JN 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2007,33(2):263-273
The authors investigated the idea that memory systems might have evolved to help us remember fitness-relevant information--specifically, information relevant to survival. In 4 incidental learning experiments, people were asked to rate common nouns for their survival relevance (e.g., in securing food, water, or protection from predators); in control conditions, the same words were rated for pleasantness, relevance to moving to a foreign land, or personal relevance. In surprise retention tests, participants consistently showed the best memory when words were rated for survival; the survival advantage held across recall, recognition, and for both within-subject and between-subjects designs. These findings suggest that memory systems are "tuned" to remember information that is processed for fitness, perhaps as a result of survival advantages accrued in the past. 相似文献
987.
Using vision, humans can accurately judge distances to locations on the ground surface up to distances of at least 20 m. Most theories of depth perception assume that this ability is associated with the fact that we live in a terrestrial world in which locations of interest often appear on the ground and for which feedback about distance is often available from nonvisual sources such as walking. Much less is known about the ability of humans to judge absolute distances to locations other than on or supported by the ground plane beyond a few meters, at which point binocular stereo provides at best limited information about distance scaling. We show that one commonly used action measure for probing absolute distance perception exhibits accurate performance, even for targets located on the ceiling of a large room. We follow this with evidence that distance to ceiling locations is recovered with a mechanism that depends, at least in part, on the angle from the line of sight to the target location and a gravity-based frame of reference. 相似文献
988.
Two studies with college student participants (n's=262, 239) examined the relation between perceptions of threat (i.e., perceptions of the probabilities and costs of future undesirable outcomes) and: (a) worry; and (b) hypothesized antecedents of perceived threat. In both studies, higher levels of worrying were associated with higher perceived probability and cost. In Study 2, the association between perceived threat and worrying remained even when taking into account maladaptive worry beliefs and the desire for predictability; in fact, the relation between worrying and worry beliefs and desire for predictability were moderated by perceptions of threat. Higher levels of perceived probability were associated with perceiving oneself and others less favorably, whereas higher levels of perceived cost were associated with higher standards. 相似文献
989.
Thompson E. DavisIII Robert E. Nida Kimberly R. Zlomke Marie S. Nebel-Schwalm 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2009,31(3):228-234
Little research has examined health-related quality of life (HRQoL) in adults with learning disabilities in post-secondary
settings and the potential relationship between a learning disability and anxiety or sadness. This study examined HRQoL in
68 undergraduate students: 34 students who reported having been diagnosed with a “learning disability” were compared to 34
students who indicated they had not been diagnosed with a learning disability. Participants completed an online survey of
anxiety, sadness, and HRQoL, including the SF-36. ANCOVAs on the Emotional Well-Being and Role Limitations Due to Emotional
Problems scales from the SF-36 revealed that students reporting a diagnosis of a learning disability were significantly more
impaired in Emotional Well-Being. Regression analyses suggested that impairment in Emotional Well-Being was mediated by separate
ratings of both anxiety and sadness. Results indicated that those undergraduates reporting learning disabilities suffered
from an impaired sense of well-being associated with anxious and sad feelings.
A portion of these data were presented at the 2005 annual meetings of the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies
(ABCT). 相似文献
990.
Trevor Thompson Tony Steffert John Gruzelier 《Personality and individual differences》2009,47(7):789-794
It is widely believed that personality has an important role in determining the effectiveness of guided imagery (GI) interventions. The primary aim of the current study was to examine whether the effect of GI on several outcome measures was dependent upon openness to experience, a theoretically relevant variable previously unexplored as a potential moderator. Thirty-five healthy participants were randomised to an animated imagery, verbal imagery or no-intervention control group, with imagery groups receiving 10 × 20 min sessions. Pre/post-assessments of cortisol, sleep, stress and creativity were administered along with the openness to experience scale. Regression analysis indicated a significant increase in cortisol and decrease in tiredness following verbal GI, but only for those high in openness. The efficacy of GI interventions may be dependent upon openness to experience and this variable should be accounted for in future studies. 相似文献