首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   56742篇
  免费   1458篇
  国内免费   16篇
  58216篇
  2020年   428篇
  2019年   547篇
  2018年   4002篇
  2017年   3401篇
  2016年   2909篇
  2015年   737篇
  2014年   752篇
  2013年   2983篇
  2012年   1774篇
  2011年   3513篇
  2010年   3171篇
  2009年   2177篇
  2008年   2736篇
  2007年   3198篇
  2006年   1033篇
  2005年   1090篇
  2004年   993篇
  2003年   895篇
  2002年   873篇
  2001年   1187篇
  2000年   1160篇
  1999年   884篇
  1998年   397篇
  1997年   391篇
  1996年   349篇
  1995年   354篇
  1993年   345篇
  1992年   733篇
  1991年   642篇
  1990年   707篇
  1989年   609篇
  1988年   643篇
  1987年   596篇
  1986年   607篇
  1985年   549篇
  1984年   507篇
  1983年   482篇
  1982年   351篇
  1979年   573篇
  1978年   404篇
  1975年   448篇
  1974年   500篇
  1973年   529篇
  1972年   397篇
  1971年   391篇
  1970年   350篇
  1969年   404篇
  1968年   473篇
  1967年   431篇
  1966年   356篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 6 毫秒
31.
32.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
33.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
34.
35.
Informed by theory and research on attributions and narrative persuasion, we compared the effectiveness of narrative and nonnarrative messages in changing attributions of responsibility for causes and solutions related to obesity in the United States. We randomly assigned 500 adults to view one of three messages (narrative, evidence, and a hybrid of the two) emphasizing environmental causes of obesity, or a no‐exposure control condition. The narrative condition increased the belief that societal actors (government, employers) are responsible for addressing obesity, but only among liberals. This pattern of results was partially explained by the success of the narrative condition in reducing reactive counterarguing, relative to the evidence condition, among liberals. We conclude with theoretical and practical implications.  相似文献   
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号