首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4417篇
  免费   207篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2021年   31篇
  2020年   56篇
  2019年   105篇
  2018年   138篇
  2017年   121篇
  2016年   127篇
  2015年   104篇
  2014年   139篇
  2013年   485篇
  2012年   171篇
  2011年   196篇
  2010年   122篇
  2009年   125篇
  2008年   181篇
  2007年   154篇
  2006年   157篇
  2005年   139篇
  2004年   115篇
  2003年   130篇
  2002年   132篇
  2001年   121篇
  2000年   102篇
  1999年   78篇
  1998年   48篇
  1997年   49篇
  1996年   49篇
  1995年   50篇
  1994年   46篇
  1993年   44篇
  1992年   58篇
  1991年   62篇
  1990年   51篇
  1989年   49篇
  1988年   38篇
  1987年   39篇
  1986年   48篇
  1985年   43篇
  1984年   43篇
  1983年   33篇
  1982年   41篇
  1981年   36篇
  1979年   40篇
  1978年   31篇
  1975年   35篇
  1974年   34篇
  1972年   31篇
  1970年   39篇
  1968年   35篇
  1967年   42篇
  1966年   33篇
排序方式: 共有4626条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
941.
942.
Following Clark Hull, it is widely considered that the asymptote of the learning curve represents motivational factors, but that rate of approach to asymptote ('i') represents associational factors. The suggestion that i may be a measure of intelligence was tested empirically in two studies. An initial exploratory study yielded a positive correlation between i and IQ. A more rigorous replication did not. The results are discussed briefly.  相似文献   
943.
944.
945.
Three-, four-, and five-year-old children's categorical and comparative understanding of high and low were examined in two experiments. Categorical knowledge was assessed by presenting subjects with a single object at varying heights (from 0 to five feet above the ground), and asking if the object was high or low. Comparative understanding of the terms was assessed by showing children two objects at a time and asking which was higher or lower. We observed two patterns of performance in children's categorical treatments: younger children in particular defined disjoint categories for high and low such that they only labelled the extreme heights as high or low, and maintained that middle heights were neither high nor low. Older children defined either-or categories such that all heights were labelled either high or low. We also found that children who defined either-or categories made correct comparative judgments across the entire range of variation whereas children who defined disjoint categories could only judge which of two objects was higher if the objects were not low (at 0 and 1 feet) and which of the objects was lower if the objects were not high (at 4 and 5 feet). The results were interpreted as reflecting a lack of appreciation that the terms are interdefined as negations of each other, and were discussed in terms of the similar semantic-congruity effects found in adults.  相似文献   
946.
This study reports the case of a 42-year-old man who suffered a ruptured aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery. His memory capabilities were assessed after a considerable recovery period during which many of his memory deficits ameliorated. His scan revealed a left frontal lesion and many of his deficits were characteristic of frontal impairment. He was impaired on temporal discrimination, and he showed marked source forgetting. He also performed badly on the Brown-Peterson task, and we suggest that this is another task that may be characteristic of frontal impairment. In contrast, the patient showed normal or near normal performance on some memory tasks but not on others. It is concluded that the patient's frontal signs are similar to those found in Korsakoff's Syndrome, but that his memory impairment is qualitatively different from that encountered in patients with the amnesic syndrome.  相似文献   
947.
948.
949.
This study examined differences in K-ABC performance between at-risk (n = 44) and normal (n = 49) preschool children. For the group at-risk, all of the mean global scores on the K-ABC clustered around a standard score of approximately 90. The normal group's mean scores were 15 to 21 standard-score points higher and clustered near 110. The results of t tests for independent samples indicated that the normal group's scores on both the global scales and K-ABC subtests were significantly higher than the at-risk group's scores in nearly every instance. Correlations among scores were consistent with previous findings for preschoolers on the K-ABC. For the normal group, correlations obtained among the global scales were nearly identical to those reported for the standardization sample. For the at-risk group, however, the correlation of .75 between Sequential processing and Achievement was higher than that (.58) between Simultaneous processing and Achievement.  相似文献   
950.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号