全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17831篇 |
免费 | 363篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
18197篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 231篇 |
2019年 | 244篇 |
2018年 | 344篇 |
2017年 | 306篇 |
2016年 | 369篇 |
2015年 | 273篇 |
2014年 | 333篇 |
2013年 | 1307篇 |
2012年 | 596篇 |
2011年 | 551篇 |
2010年 | 386篇 |
2009年 | 342篇 |
2008年 | 481篇 |
2007年 | 509篇 |
2006年 | 433篇 |
2005年 | 429篇 |
2004年 | 373篇 |
2003年 | 326篇 |
2002年 | 354篇 |
2001年 | 709篇 |
2000年 | 695篇 |
1999年 | 490篇 |
1998年 | 192篇 |
1997年 | 186篇 |
1996年 | 174篇 |
1995年 | 148篇 |
1992年 | 375篇 |
1991年 | 346篇 |
1990年 | 363篇 |
1989年 | 307篇 |
1988年 | 291篇 |
1987年 | 289篇 |
1986年 | 267篇 |
1985年 | 261篇 |
1984年 | 250篇 |
1983年 | 210篇 |
1982年 | 162篇 |
1979年 | 224篇 |
1978年 | 174篇 |
1977年 | 152篇 |
1975年 | 194篇 |
1974年 | 233篇 |
1973年 | 232篇 |
1972年 | 184篇 |
1971年 | 182篇 |
1970年 | 152篇 |
1969年 | 176篇 |
1968年 | 210篇 |
1967年 | 192篇 |
1966年 | 156篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
Comparing interspersed requests and social comments as antecedents for increasing student compliance
Two students were alternately presented with interspersed high-compliance requests and social comments as antecedents to low-compliance requests. An initial comparison demonstrated similar positive effects on compliance for interspersed requests and social comments. A second analysis indicated that the effectiveness of social comments for increasing compliance was related to the time interval between social comments and low-compliance requests. 相似文献
993.
Frank T. Birtel 《Zygon》1995,30(2):315-327
Abstract. An attempt to discover what can be learned from the recent work of Frank Tipler on the Omega Point theory requires an analysis of his framework of understanding from scientific, philosophical, and theological perspectives. A critique of his crucial ideas, and of the salient points raised by some of his critics, can then be undertaken within the compass of his strengths. A critique of the critiques of Tipler's work allows one to evaluate the extent and limitations of his contributions. 相似文献
994.
Deniz S. Ones Frank L. Schmidt Chokalingam Viswesvaran David T. Lykken 《Journal of business and psychology》1996,10(4):487-501
In the August 1993 issue ofJournal of Applied Psychology, Deniz Ones, Vish Viswesvaran, and Frank Schmidt published a monograph on the validity of integrity tests as revealed by meta-analysis. They concluded that integrity tests have substantial and generalizable predictive validity for criteria of both overall job performance and counterproductive behaviors on the job (e.g., disciplinary problems, absenteeism, rule breaking, violence on the job, theft, etc.). During the Fall of 1992, the authors sent a preprint of this study to David Lykken of the University of Minnesota, stimulating the exchange of letters that is reproduced here. The doubts, concerns, and feelings about integrity tests expressed by Professor Lykken are likely to be shared by others. The responses by Professors Schmidt, Ones, and Viswesvaran attempt to show that these worries are unfounded. 相似文献
995.
Rev. William W. Rich S.T.M. Stephen Kurtz C.S.W. Barry Ulanov Aryeh Maidenbaum Ph.D. Mark Finn Ph.D. Claude Barbre Catherine E. Cook Robert Gunn D. Min. Elizabeth Tener C.S.W. W. Brown M.S.W. C.S.W. Jill Carlen Kirby M.S. C.S.W. Elisabeth M. Smith M.P.S. Kathleen Ford S.T.M. Angelo Paiano Antoinette Goodwin Ph.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1996,35(1):67-88
996.
997.
Berry DT Adams JJ Clark CD Thacker SR Burger TL Wetter MW Baer RA 《Journal of personality assessment》1996,67(1):26-36
We compared Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI-2) results from 2 groups of mildly to moderately disturbed psychiatric outpatients (N=60) answering under either Standard or Cry for Help instructions. Results from previously completed intake MMPI-2 protocols were obtained for each participant. The two groups were comparable on intake MMPI-2 variables and demographic characteristics. Analysis of feedback data from the second, experimental observation indicated that the two groups had equivalent self-estimates of understanding of their role and success in simulating their role. Significant differences were found between the two groups on their experimental MMPI-2 protocols. The Cry for Help group had significantly higher scores on F, F-K, Fb, Ds2, and Fp scales as well as significantly lower scorns on the K scale. The Cry for Help group also had significantly higher scores on all clinical scales with the exception of 5, which was not tested. Cutting scores derived by Rogers, Sewell, and Ustad (1995) for the detection of a Cry for Help in outpatients were cross-validated with fairly positive results. Although these results are promising, particularly in light of the shrinkage expected on cross-validation, further research is needed in this area. 相似文献
998.
Eight pigeons were trained on a delayed presence-versus-absence discrimination paradigm in which a sample stimulus was presented on some trials but not on others. If a sample was presented, then a response to one choice key produced food. If no sample was presented, a response to the other choice key produced food. The basic finding was that performance remained constant and well above 50% correct on no-sample trials as the retention interval increased, whereas performance dropped precipitously (to below 50% correct) on sample trials. In the second phase of the experiment, all of the trials were no-sample trials, and reinforcers were delivered probabilistically for one group of pigeons and according to time-based schedules for the other group. The exact reinforcement probabilities used in Phase 2 were those calculated to be in effect on no-sample trials in Phase 1 (according to a discrete-state model of performance). Subjects did not show exclusive preference for the richer alternative on no-sample trials in the first phase, but those in the probabilistic group developed near-exclusive preference for the richer alternative during the second phase. These data are inconsistent with the predictions of the discrete-state model, but are easily accommodated by an account based on signal detection theory, which also can be applied effectively to discrimination of event duration and the “subjective shortening” effect. 相似文献
999.
R A Bauman T G Raslear S R Hursh D Shurtleff L Simmons 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1996,65(2):401-422
Three experiments were conducted to study the effect of an imperfect substitute for food on demand for food in a closed economy. In Experiments 1 and 2, rats pressed a lever for their entire daily food ration, and a fixed ratio of presses was required for each food pellet. In both experiments, the fixed ratio was held constant during a daily session but was increased between sessions. The fixed ratio was increased over a series of daily sessions once in the absence of concurrently available sucrose and again when sucrose pellets were freely available. For both series, increases in the fixed ratio reduced food intake, but body weight was reduced only in the no-sucrose condition. In the sucrose condition, body weight and total caloric intake (sucrose plus food) were relatively unaffected by increases in the fixed ratio. At all fixed ratios, food intake was proportionally reduced by the intake of sucrose. In Experiment 3, monkeys obtained food or saccharin by pressing keys; the fixed ratio of presses per food pellet was increased once when tap water was each monkey's only source of fluid, again when each monkey's water was sweetened with saccharin, and a third time when each monkey had concurrent access to the saccharin solution and plain water. Increases in the fixed ratio, but not the intake of the saccharin solution, reduced each monkey's food intake. Because neither rats' sucrose nor monkeys' saccharin intakes affected the slope of the respective demand curves for food, monkeys and rats increased their daily output of presses and thereby defended their daily intake of those complementary elements of food. However, sucrose reduced rats' food intake. The relative constancy of body weight and total caloric intake in the sucrose condition is consistent with the possibility that rats tended to regulate caloric intake. 相似文献
1000.
Abstract— The evaluative categorizations that underlie affective and attitudinal judgments have often been equated with non-evaluative categorizations despite the central importance of evaluative processes for survival In the present experiment, a late positive potential (LPP) of the event-related brain potential elicited when participants evaluatively categorized food items as positive or nonpositive was compared with the LPP elicited when participants semantically (i e., nonevaluatively) categorized food items as vegetable or nonvegetable Results revealed that evaluative categorizations evoked an LPP that was relatively larger over the right than the left scalp regions compared with the LPP evoked by nonevaluative categorizations. This finding provides evidence regarding the differences in neural and cognitive processes involved in evaluative and nonevaluative categorizations. 相似文献