全文获取类型
收费全文 | 23061篇 |
免费 | 369篇 |
国内免费 | 705篇 |
专业分类
24135篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 52篇 |
2022年 | 89篇 |
2021年 | 99篇 |
2020年 | 135篇 |
2019年 | 103篇 |
2018年 | 3585篇 |
2017年 | 2907篇 |
2016年 | 2351篇 |
2015年 | 291篇 |
2014年 | 182篇 |
2013年 | 291篇 |
2012年 | 737篇 |
2011年 | 2624篇 |
2010年 | 2719篇 |
2009年 | 1666篇 |
2008年 | 1928篇 |
2007年 | 2469篇 |
2006年 | 336篇 |
2005年 | 431篇 |
2004年 | 303篇 |
2003年 | 200篇 |
2002年 | 136篇 |
2001年 | 69篇 |
2000年 | 95篇 |
1999年 | 52篇 |
1998年 | 44篇 |
1997年 | 40篇 |
1996年 | 24篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
速度、步幅与窗口对引导式中文文本阅读工效的影响 总被引:3,自引:1,他引:3
引导式显示是在视觉显示终端(VDT)上呈现动态文本的主要方式之一.本研究考察了显示窗口大小、跳动步幅、显示速度等因素对引导式中文文本的阅读工效的影响.实验中采用了3(跳动步幅,被试内变量)×3(显示速度,被试内变量)×2(显示窗口大小,被试间变量)的混合设计,共有18个被试参加了实验.结果发现(1)跳动步幅、显示速度和窗口大小均影响中文文本的阅读工效;(2)达到最佳阅读工效的实验条件在速度和步幅上分别集中于200~400字/分和1~3字/步.最后本文结合中文阅读的相关眼动研究,对这些结果的成因进行了简要的探讨. 相似文献
82.
本研究选取发展良好与适应不良的三——五年级小学生为被试,以问卷访谈形式调查他们心目中的正性和负性意义事件。结果表明,两组被试在事件意义体验的人际亲和性、内在一外在性和丰富性方面有明显不同的反映。感受的丰富性表现在正负性意义事件两方面,某种事件负性意义感受的出现是良好适应的表现。 相似文献
83.
个体解决三项系列问题的心理模型 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
该研究设计了2种实验条件,对360名6-19岁个体解决三项系列问题的能力发展进行了研究,旨在讨论个体解决三项系列问题的心理模型的发展问题,结果表明:(1)6-19岁个体对三项系列问题中有必然逻辑答案的题目的判断能力的发展速度比较平稳;(2)个体对三项系列问题中有必然逻辑答案的题目的判断能力先于对没有逻辑答案的题目,6-9岁个体对三项系列问题中没有必然逻辑答案的题目的判断能力的发展速度低于10岁以后的个体;(3)空间模型、语义模型、语义一空间混合模型和枢纽项比较模型是相互补充的,是在不同认知发展阶段依一定次序,按一定的阶段逐渐形成的,经历了5个不同的发展阶段,最终形成以枢纽项比较模型为核心、四种心理模型并存于个体的心理结构之中的复合模型。 相似文献
84.
Jonathan M. Keefe Jocelyn L. Sy Frank Tong David H. Zald 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2001,81(1):205-216
The emotional attentional blink (EAB) refers to a temporary impairment in the ability to identify a target when it is preceded by an emotional distractor. It is thought to occur because the emotional salience of the distractor exogenously captures attention for a brief duration, rendering the target unattended and preventing it from reaching awareness. Here we tested the extent to which the EAB can be attenuated by inducing a diffuse top-down attentional state, which has been shown to improve target identification in an analogous attentional phenomenon, the attentional blink. Rapid sequences of landscape images were presented centrally, and participants reported the orientation of a ± 90° rotation of a landscape target. To induce a diffuse state of attention, participants were given a secondary task of monitoring for the appearance of a colored dot in the periphery. We found that emotional distractors impaired target recognition performance to comparable extents, regardless of whether or not participants concurrently performed the peripheral-monitoring task. Moreover, we found that performance of the secondary task led to an impaired ability to ignore neutral distractors. Subjective ratings of target vividness mirrored the behavioral accuracy, with frequent reports of intermediate levels of vividness suggesting that the EAB might impair target visibility in a graded manner. Our results demonstrate that the EAB is robust to manipulations of top-down attention, suggesting that the temporary capture of attention by emotionally salient stimuli involves processes that are distinct from those that produce the attentional blink. 相似文献
85.
Baguley T 《Behavior research methods》2012,44(1):158-175
The psychological and statistical literature contains several proposals for calculating and plotting confidence intervals
(CIs) for within-subjects (repeated measures) ANOVA designs. A key distinction is between intervals supporting inference about
patterns of means (and differences between pairs of means, in particular) and those supporting inferences about individual
means. In this report, it is argued that CIs for the former are best accomplished by adapting intervals proposed by Cousineau
(Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45, 2005) and Morey (Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61–64, 2008) so that nonoverlapping CIs for individual means correspond to a confidence for their difference that does not include zero.
CIs for the latter can be accomplished by fitting a multilevel model. In situations in which both types of inference are of
interest, the use of a two-tiered CI is recommended. Free, open-source, cross-platform software for such interval estimates
and plots (and for some common alternatives) is provided in the form of R functions for one-way within-subjects and two-way
mixed ANOVA designs. These functions provide an easy-to-use solution to the difficult problem of calculating and displaying
within-subjects CIs. 相似文献
86.
Welch’s (Biometrika 29: 350–362, 1938) procedure has emerged as a robust alternative to the Student’s t test for comparing the means of two normal populations with unknown and possibly unequal variances. To facilitate the advocated
statistical practice of confidence intervals and further improve the potential applicability of Welch’s procedure, in the
present article, we consider exact approaches to optimize sample size determinations for precise interval estimation of the
difference between two means under various allocation and cost considerations. The desired precision of a confidence interval
is assessed with respect to the control of expected half-width, and to the assurance probability of interval half-width within
a designated value. Furthermore, the design schemes in terms of participant allocation and cost constraints include (a) giving
the ratio of group sizes, (b) specifying one sample size, (c) attaining maximum precision performance for a fixed cost, and
(d) meeting a specified precision level for the least cost. The proposed methods provide useful alternatives to the conventional
sample size procedures. Also, the developed programs expand the degree of generality for the existing statistical software
packages and can be accessed at brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/ supplemental. 相似文献
87.
Køppe’s proposition of four layers in theoretical building are used here in the exploration of a specific case of eclectic combination, the use of Piagetian and Vygotskian general approaches to the analysis of proportional reasoning as a cognitive mathematical ability. It is proposed here that the eclectic consideration of these contributions depends on the consideration of their specificity, in the sense that they highlight different aspects of the phenomenon under scrutiny, and also on the consideration of the coherence between this eclectic convergence and premises in terms of schools of thought under which each contribution is framed. We conclude proposing in accordance to S. Køppe’s proposal that eclecticism can be valuable and heuristic in theory development, but this contribution will depend largely on the effort in establishing careful relations between the four layers of theory-building. 相似文献
88.
Externalizing psychopathology (EXT) is a framework for understanding diagnostic comorbidity and etiology of antisocial and
substance-use behaviors. EXT indicates continuity in adulthood but the structure of adolescent EXT is less clear. This report
examines whether adolescent EXT is trait-like, as has been found with adults, or categorical. We use tests of measurement
invariance to determine how diagnostic indicators of EXT differ in adolescents compared to adults. The EXT measures employed
were DSM-IIIR diagnoses of adult antisocial behavior, conduct disorder, and alcohol, marijuana, and drug dependence. Latent
trait, latent class, and hybrid models were fit to two separate data sets: 2,769 seventeen-year-old adolescents and 2,619
adults from the Minnesota Twin Family Study. The best model in both samples was a single-trait LT model. Parameters from the
adolescent and adult models were equivalent for all disorders except alcohol dependence. It appears that EXT in adolescence
can be accurately represented by a single-trait model, and the measurement properties of EXT are similar during these time
periods with the exception of alcohol dependence. 相似文献
89.
Examined a cognitive-behavioral pathway by which depressive symptoms in mothers and fathers increase risk for later child externalizing problem behavior via parents’ appraisals of child behavior and physical discipline. Participants were 245 children (118 girls) at risk for school-age conduct problems, and their parents and teachers. Children were approximately 3 years old at Time 1 (T1) and 5 ½ years old at Time 2 (T2). At T1, mothers and fathers reported their depressive symptoms, perceptions of their child’s reciprocal affection and responsiveness, frequency of physical punishment, and child externalizing problems. Mothers, fathers, and teachers provided ratings of externalizing behavior at T2. Structural equation modeling revealed that parents’ negative attributions mediated positive relations between their depressive symptoms and frequency of physical punishment for both fathers and mothers. More frequent physical punishment, in turn, predicted increased child externalizing behavior at T2. In future research, transactional mechanisms underlying effects of clinical depression on child conduct problems should be explored at multiple stages of development. For parents showing depressive symptoms, restructuring distorted perceptions about their children’s behavior may be an important component of intervention programs. 相似文献
90.
Relatively little is known about the potential interplay between genetic and environmental influences on attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD), including gene-environment interaction (G×E). There is evidence that parenting behavior interacts with offspring
genotype in the development of externalizing problems, but studies have largely focused on explicit maltreatment rather than
differentiated measures of parenting behavior, including positive and negative parenting. We tested the interactive effects
of the 30-base pair variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase A gene (MAO-A) with positive
and negative parenting behavior on parent- and teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity symptoms among 150 6–9 year-old
boys with and without ADHD. Negative parenting predicted parent and teacher ratings of inattention symptoms, but only among
boys with high-activity MAO-A genotype. MAO-A genotype did not moderate the association of positive parenting and parent-
and teacher ratings of ADHD. We discuss the potential role of interactive exchanges between parenting behavior and child genotype
in the development and persistence of ADHD and related behavior problems. 相似文献