全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3436篇 |
免费 | 63篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3501篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2023年 | 22篇 |
2021年 | 37篇 |
2020年 | 67篇 |
2019年 | 88篇 |
2018年 | 86篇 |
2017年 | 114篇 |
2016年 | 110篇 |
2015年 | 84篇 |
2014年 | 91篇 |
2013年 | 347篇 |
2012年 | 146篇 |
2011年 | 170篇 |
2010年 | 124篇 |
2009年 | 119篇 |
2008年 | 126篇 |
2007年 | 128篇 |
2006年 | 129篇 |
2005年 | 104篇 |
2004年 | 105篇 |
2003年 | 119篇 |
2002年 | 109篇 |
2001年 | 58篇 |
2000年 | 51篇 |
1999年 | 46篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 41篇 |
1996年 | 44篇 |
1995年 | 36篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 35篇 |
1992年 | 40篇 |
1991年 | 37篇 |
1990年 | 45篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 38篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 35篇 |
1983年 | 42篇 |
1982年 | 29篇 |
1981年 | 29篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1973年 | 28篇 |
1968年 | 22篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有3501条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
Perceived heaviness has been shown to be specific to an object’s rotational inertia (I ), its resistance to rotational acceleration.
According to the kinematic specification of dynamics (KSD) principle, we hypothesized that I is optically specified by rotational
kinematics. Using virtual depictions of wielded objects, we investigated whether the visually detected rotational kinematics
of wielded objects would influence perceived heaviness in a manner consistent with the inertial model of heaviness perception.
We scaled the virtual object’s movement so that it rotated more or less than its wielded counterpart, specifying lower and
higher I, respectively. Perceived heaviness was inversely related to the rotational scaling factor, consistent with a KSD
interpretation of the inertial model. 相似文献
172.
Using a speeded retrieval procedure, we investigated time-of-day effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Morning-type
adults were tested at either peak (early morning) or off-peak (late afternoon) times on a speeded implicit (Experiment 1)
or explicit (Experiment 2) stem completion task. In Experiment 1, retrieval strategies were identified by changes in response
speed between a practice phase with rapid retrieval and an implicit memory test phase. Performance based on controlled retrieval
(shown by slowdown participants) showed more priming at peak than at off-peak times of day, a finding confirmed in Experiment
2, in which the participants were given intentional retrieval instructions when the materials switched. In contrast, performance
based on automatic retrieval (shown by nonslowdown participants) did not differ across peak and off-peak times. The finding
suggests a robust synchrony effect in controlled retrieval, but not in automatic retrieval, which does not appear to vary
across the day. 相似文献
173.
Most body-image research has focused on the trait level of body-image evaluation, often neglecting the momentary fluctuations many people experience in everyday life. The present prospective study investigated whether theory-relevant body-image measures, perfectionistic self-presentation, and eating attitudes would predict average day-to-day body-image levels and their intra-individual variability. A convenience sample consisted of 121 women from two universities. In Phase 1 of the study, participants completed an online battery of selected body-image and personality questionnaires. In Phase 2, participants went online to complete the dependent measure, the Body Image States Scale, once per evening over 10 days. As hypothesized, more favorable body-image state levels were associated with less investment in appearance for self-worth, less body-image disturbance, fewer body-image cognitive distortions, less disturbed eating attitudes, and lower body mass. Moreover, greater day-to-day body-image variability was predicted by greater psychological investment in appearance, more body-image cognitive distortions, and higher perfectionistic self-presentation. Implications and future directions for research are discussed. 相似文献
174.
175.
176.
Kevin Rathunde 《Journal of Adult Development》1995,2(3):159-172
This article explores the connections between wisdom-related attributes and abiding interest and draws upon material from interviews with three eminent historians in later life. It is proposed that one characteristic of wisdom, namely, the coordination of affective/attaching and cognitive/detaching modes of processing information, is related to abiding interest or the ability to self-regulate attention in a way that promotes lifelong learning. The article (a) reviews pertinent constructs in the literatures on adult development and wisdom; (b) presents selected excerpts from in-depth interviews; and (c) concludes by discussing the developmental implications of the ideas presented. 相似文献
177.
Product manuals have been virtually ignored in published research despite their relevance to consumer satisfaction and marketplace safety. This study examines self-efficacy (perceptions of capabilities related to operating a product) and how it influences the effectiveness of and is also influenced by three safety message formats commonly used in product manuals (i.e., 1. safety messages presented in list format at the front of the manual; 2; safety messages presented by semantic similarity interspersed throughout the manual—chunked format, and 3. safety messages presented in a combined format—both list and chunked). As anticipated, pre-exposure self-efficacy was a significant covariate which was found to be positively related to post-exposure self-efficacy and negatively related to information evaluation. Further, controlling for pre-exposure self-efficacy, post-exposure self-efficacy was significantly lower in the combined (list plus chunked) condition. Findings of this research have important implications for the practice of repeating safety warnings and future research in the consumer information domain.Authors are listed in alphabetical order. This study was supported by an Illinois State University Research Grant. 相似文献
178.
Michelle Bidaut-Russell Wendy Reich Linda B. Cottler Lee N. Robins Wilson M. Compton Richard E. Mattison 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1995,23(5):641-659
To identify reasons for discrepancies between parent and child reports of child/adolescents's psychiatric symptoms, parents and adolescents (51 pairs) were asked to guess what the other would answer to questions from the PC-DISC about the adolescent's psychiatric symptoms, and to explain why they expected disagreement when the answer they provided for the other was different from their own. Adolescents' explanations for expecting (1) parental denial of symptoms the adolescent reported were: the parent was unaware of, forgot about, assumed the adolescent could not have, or trivialized the symptom; and (2) parental report of symptoms the adolescent denied were: the parent misread or exaggerated the adolescent's symptom, had too high expectations for the adolescent's behavior, put a negative label on or did not trust the adolescent. Parents' reasons for expecting their children to (1) deny symptoms the parents reported were: the adolescent did not remember how s/he felt, lied, did not recognize or minimized the importance or frequency of the symptom; and (2) report symptoms the parents denied were: the adolescent lied, exaggerated the importance of or interpreted the symptom differently.Support for this work was provided by National Institute of Drug Abuse grant number DA-05585 (Dr. Cottler, P.I.), and National Institute of Mental Health grants numbers MH-31302 (Dr. R. C. Cloninger, P.I.) and MH-17104 (Dr. Cottler, P.I.).Address all correspondence, including reprint requests, to Michelle Bidaut-Russell, Ph.D, MPH, MPE, Department of Psychiatry, Box 8134, Washington University School of Medicine, 4940 Children's Place, St. Louis, Missouri 63110. 相似文献
179.
180.
Stimulus categorization by brain-damaged patients 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Patients with localized insult to the right hemisphere, or to either the anterior or the posterior portion of the left hemisphere, as well as neurologically intact controls, evaluated stimuli on a seven-point rating scale for their degree of category membership. The stimuli were taken from one of two continua, one composed of fruit and vegetable items, and the other of items differing in hue and shape. Different subsets of stimuli provided different contexts for the judgments of category membership. The two left-hemisphere groups showed anomalies in categorizing the fruit and vegetable items but not the perceptual items, while the reverse was true for the right-hemisphere patients. Moreover, both left-hemisphere groups demonstrated context effects in their judgments of the representativeness of the fruit and vegetable items, but differed in the way in which they responded to changing contexts. Left posterior patients demonstrated weak category boundaries and even reclassified items. In contrast, patients with left anterior damage showed highly categorical responses and less differentiation of items within a category. All groups showed striking context effects in judgments of perceptual items in terms of changes in representativeness ratings and the location of a category boundary. Alternative interpretations of the results are offered. 相似文献