全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12704篇 |
免费 | 621篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
13330篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 110篇 |
2020年 | 191篇 |
2019年 | 202篇 |
2018年 | 309篇 |
2017年 | 295篇 |
2016年 | 297篇 |
2015年 | 226篇 |
2014年 | 259篇 |
2013年 | 1010篇 |
2012年 | 427篇 |
2011年 | 469篇 |
2010年 | 299篇 |
2009年 | 251篇 |
2008年 | 365篇 |
2007年 | 368篇 |
2006年 | 338篇 |
2005年 | 281篇 |
2004年 | 262篇 |
2003年 | 249篇 |
2002年 | 264篇 |
2001年 | 441篇 |
2000年 | 408篇 |
1999年 | 293篇 |
1998年 | 129篇 |
1997年 | 108篇 |
1996年 | 119篇 |
1995年 | 115篇 |
1993年 | 111篇 |
1992年 | 227篇 |
1991年 | 229篇 |
1990年 | 221篇 |
1989年 | 213篇 |
1988年 | 221篇 |
1987年 | 188篇 |
1986年 | 182篇 |
1985年 | 179篇 |
1984年 | 160篇 |
1983年 | 122篇 |
1982年 | 126篇 |
1979年 | 153篇 |
1975年 | 140篇 |
1974年 | 158篇 |
1973年 | 183篇 |
1972年 | 155篇 |
1971年 | 141篇 |
1970年 | 124篇 |
1969年 | 134篇 |
1968年 | 178篇 |
1967年 | 132篇 |
1966年 | 141篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
In the model of motion perception proposed by Swanston, Wade, and Day (1987, Perception 16 143-159) it was suggested that retinocentric motion and eye movement information are combined independently for each eye, to give left and right orbitocentric representations of movement. The weighted orbitocentric values are then added, to give a single agocentric representation. It is shown that for a physical motion observed without pursuit eye movements this formulation predicts a reduction in the perceived extent of motion with monocular as opposed to binocular viewing. This prediction was tested, and shown to be incorrect. Accordingly, a modification of the model is proposed, in which the left and right retinocentric signals are weighted according to the presence or absence of stimulation, and combined to give a binocular retinocentric representation. In a similar way left-eye and right-eye position signals are combined to give a single binocular eye movement signal for version. This is then added to the binocular retinocentric signal to give the egocentric representation. This modification provides a unified account of both static visual direction and movement perception. 相似文献
992.
Questionnaires were mailed to a random sample of administrators of 1,080 long-term care facilities, in order to obtain information on the extent and nature of overt suicide and intentional life-threatening behavior (ILTB). Rates were calculated for death for overt suicides and ILTB. In-depth case studies, involving observation, interviews, and examination of medical records, were conducted in four facilities. Quantitative analysis revealed that white males were most at risk. Refusing to eat or drink and refusing medications were the most common suicidal behaviors. Depression, loneliness, feelings of family rejection, and loss were significant factors. 相似文献
993.
R. A. Boakes Mark N.O. Davies Maggie Redshaw R.I.M. Dunbar R. A. Boakes 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1991,43(4):449-458
Buckley, Key W. (1989). Mechanical man: John Broadeus Watson and the beginnings of Behaviorism. New York: Guilford Press. Pp. 293. ISBN 0-89862-744-3. $32.45/£18.00.
Colgan, P. (1989), Animal Motivation. New York and London: Chapman and Hall. Pp. 168. ISBN 0412-31850. £25.00 (hardback). ISBN 0412-31860, £10.95 (paperback).
Antinucci, F (Ed.) (1989). Cognitive structure and development in nonhuman prinates. Hillsdale, Nj: Laweence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Pp. 266. ISBN 0-8058-0544. £16.50 (Hardback).
Parker, S.T. & Gibson, K.R. (Eds.) (1990). “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp. 590. ISBN 0-521-38028-6. £40.00 (hardback).
Ley, Ronald (1990). A whisper of espionnage: Wolfgang Koehler and the apes of Tenerife. Garden City Park, NY: Avery Publoshing Corporation. Pp.264. ISBN 0-89529-432-x. $19.95. 相似文献
Colgan, P. (1989), Animal Motivation. New York and London: Chapman and Hall. Pp. 168. ISBN 0412-31850. £25.00 (hardback). ISBN 0412-31860, £10.95 (paperback).
Antinucci, F (Ed.) (1989). Cognitive structure and development in nonhuman prinates. Hillsdale, Nj: Laweence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Pp. 266. ISBN 0-8058-0544. £16.50 (Hardback).
Parker, S.T. & Gibson, K.R. (Eds.) (1990). “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp. 590. ISBN 0-521-38028-6. £40.00 (hardback).
Ley, Ronald (1990). A whisper of espionnage: Wolfgang Koehler and the apes of Tenerife. Garden City Park, NY: Avery Publoshing Corporation. Pp.264. ISBN 0-89529-432-x. $19.95. 相似文献
994.
995.
Daniel N. Osherson Joshua Stern Ormond Wilkie Michael Stob Edward E. Smith 《Cognitive Science》1991,15(2):251-269
A probability may be called “default” if it is neither derived from preestablished probabilities nor based on considerations of frequency or symmetry. Default probabilities presumably arise through reasoning based on causality and similarity. This article advances a model of default probability based on a featural approach to similarity. The accuracy of the model is assessed by comparing its predictions to the probabilities provided by undergraduates asked to reason about mammals. 相似文献
996.
A Biblarz R M Brown D N Biblarz M Pilgrim B F Baldree 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》1991,21(4):374-384
This experiment investigated the effects of viewing films on suicide and violence on attitudes toward suicide. Non-suicidal university students were assigned to one of three groups: One group was shown a film about suicide, a second saw a film about violence, and the third saw a neutral film. Perceptions of the appropriateness of suicide and level of emotional arousal were measured by a questionnaire administered before and after the films were shown. Both the suicide and violent film groups showed increases in arousal scores after viewing the films. The results suggest that arousal became linked to cognitions specific to film content as a result of viewing the films dealing with suicide and violence. Other findings suggest that, for the suicide and violent film groups, perceived relations with parents were important predictors of film-induced changes in attitudes toward suicide. 相似文献
997.
998.
999.
1000.
J N Vickers 《Perception》1992,21(1):117-132
The gaze of low and higher handicap golfers was assessed while they performed consecutive putts from 3 m, wearing an eye-movement helmet that permitted normal mobility. MANOVA (count and duration), with univariate follow-up, revealed significant differences in gaze between five low (LH, 0-8) and seven higher handicap golfers (HH, 10-16). The LH (ie more highly skilled) golfers were found to use a variable form of gaze control in which longer fixation durations on the ball and target were observed, and there were fewer fixations on the club and surface, with more express saccades and quicker saccades between gaze locations. The HH golfers, in contrast, allocated the same mean durations to each gaze (about 1 s), independent of type of control (fixation, saccade, or tracking) or location (ball, club, target, or surface). In comparing hits to misses, there was an increased probability of hits if the golfers used express saccades to the club during preparation, and a steady fixation on the ball during the backswing/foreswing of the club, as well as a steady fixation on the surface during contact. These results suggest that with the acquisition of the putting skill, there are changes in gaze control, characterized by economy in the number of gaze shifts, the development of priority to specific gaze locations, and economy in the allocation of time between preferred gaze locations. In the discussion two reasons are proposed that may partially explain the results found here, as well as help further our understanding of the role of gaze in targetting skills. 相似文献