全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2585篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2624篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 25篇 |
2023年 | 16篇 |
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 32篇 |
2020年 | 69篇 |
2019年 | 79篇 |
2018年 | 79篇 |
2017年 | 95篇 |
2016年 | 87篇 |
2015年 | 65篇 |
2014年 | 83篇 |
2013年 | 275篇 |
2012年 | 118篇 |
2011年 | 125篇 |
2010年 | 100篇 |
2009年 | 88篇 |
2008年 | 92篇 |
2007年 | 106篇 |
2006年 | 93篇 |
2005年 | 86篇 |
2004年 | 75篇 |
2003年 | 85篇 |
2002年 | 84篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 37篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 26篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2624条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Thomas?A.?Fergus Kevin?D.?WuEmail author 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2011,33(4):446-458
Using two large nonclinical samples (N = 725), relations between five targeted cognitive variables [intolerance of uncertainty, negative problem orientation, perfectionism/certainty,
responsibility/threat estimation, and importance/control of thoughts] and mood [depression] and anxiety [social anxiety, generalized
anxiety, and obsessive–compulsive] symptoms were examined. Analyses provided multiple levels of specificity, including zero-order
correlations, partial correlations controlling for the effects of positive and negative affect, regression analyses, and hierarchical
structural modeling. Results were that (a) intolerance of uncertainty showed relative specificity to anxiety versus depression
symptoms and (b) negative problem orientation was common to mood and anxiety symptoms. Although certain analyses suggested
that (c) perfectionism/certainty specifically predicted generalized anxiety and (d) both responsibility/threat estimation
and importance/control of thoughts were unique predictors of obsessive–compulsive symptoms, these three cognitive variables
inconsistently predicted symptom scores across the two studies. Conceptual and therapeutic implications are discussed. 相似文献
132.
Kevin Anderson 《Studies in East European Thought》1992,44(2):79-129
I would like to thank the following people for helpful comments and criticisms: Janet Afary, Paul Buhle, Bud Burkhard, Olga Domanski, Nigel Gibson, Peter Hudis, Martin Jay, Douglas Kellner, Andrew Kliman, Pierre Lantz, Heinz Osterle, Jim Thomas, Lou Turner, and Alan Wald. 相似文献
133.
Resource-limited regions of the world represent the areas most affected by the global HIV epidemic. Currently, there are insufficient
data on the neurocognitive effects of HIV in these areas and neuropsychological studies that have been carried out thus far
are marked by inconsistent methods, test batteries, and rating systems for levels of cognitive impairment. These differences
in methods, along with genetic variability of both virus and host, differences in co-infections and other co-morbidities,
differences in language and culture, and infrastructural deficiencies in many international settings create challenges to
the assessment of neurocognitive functioning and interpretation of neuropsychological data. Identifying neurocognitive impairment
directly attributable to HIV, exploring relationships between HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment, disease variables,
and everyday functioning, evaluating differences in HIV-1 subtype associated neuropathology, and determining implications
for treatment remain complicated and challenging goals. Endeavors to establish a more standardized approach to neurocognitive
assessments across international studies in addition to accumulating appropriate normative data that will allow more accurate
rating of neuropsychological test performance will be crucial to future efforts attempting to achieve these goals. 相似文献
134.
Kevin Hart 《Sophia》2009,48(4):435-459
This essay seeks to explore contemplation as it features in Christian theology and philosophy, both ancient and modern. Contemplation,
in ancient philosophy, is transformed in Christian theology; nonetheless, it has the structure of what Jean Wahl calls ‘transascendance’,
a rising to the heights. Although contemplation remains as a theme in modern Christian theology, it drops out in modern philosophy:
that is, post-Renaissance philosophy. And yet it returns, both in analytic and continental philosophy, in the twentieth century.
It returns, however, in the mode of ‘transdescendance’: by way of conditions of possibility, and fundamental orientations. 相似文献
135.
Based on the standard error of measurement, Holland (1985) suggested the “rule of eight” for determining the meaningfulness of differences between two summary scores on the Self Directed Search. The present study empirically examined the rule's validity for practice. The participants were 2397 (1497 females and 900 males) undergraduate students enrolled in the exploratory major at a large Midwestern university. The results strongly supported the interpretive rule of eight. For those participants who scored a primary-code distinction of eight or more points, the hit rate was 55% with a kappa of .38. This compared to a hit rate of 40% with a kappa of .21 for those participants with less than eight-point distinction between their top two scores. The authors also discuss the practical implications for interpreting SDS results using the “rule of eight.” 相似文献
136.
Chambless DL Sharpless BA Rodriguez D McCarthy KS Milrod BL Khalsa SR Barber JP 《Behavior Therapy》2011,42(4):689-699
Aims of this study were (a) to summarize the psychometric literature on the Mobility Inventory for Agoraphobia (MIA), (b) to examine the convergent and discriminant validity of the MIA's Avoidance Alone and Avoidance Accompanied rating scales relative to clinical severity ratings of anxiety disorders from the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule (ADIS), and (c) to establish a cutoff score indicative of interviewers’ diagnosis of agoraphobia for the Avoidance Alone scale. A meta-analytic synthesis of 10 published studies yielded positive evidence for internal consistency and convergent and discriminant validity of the scales. Participants in the present study were 129 people with a diagnosis of panic disorder. Internal consistency was excellent for this sample, α = .95 for AAC and .96 for AAL. When the MIA scales were correlated with interviewer ratings, evidence for convergent and discriminant validity for AAL was strong (convergent r with agoraphobia severity ratings = .63 vs. discriminant rs of .10–.29 for other anxiety disorders) and more modest but still positive for AAC (.54 vs. .01–.37). Receiver operating curve analysis indicated that the optimal operating point for AAL as an indicator of ADIS agoraphobia diagnosis was 1.61, which yielded sensitivity of .87 and specificity of .73. 相似文献
137.
The focus of this paper is mechanisms that may be responsible for intellectual and developmental differences in the cognitive strategies of typical and atypical children, including those with mental retardation. The discussion of these mechanisms is based on behavioral experiments on external memory strategies and on a set of neural network models designed for these tasks. Following the review of the external memory research, the rationale for using neural network models, how they have been used in other research, and their specific application to intellectual and developmental differences in external memory, including the results of several simulations, are reviewed. This is followed by a discussion of the mechanisms of intellectual differences and developmental change included in the models and some challenges for this type of modeling. Neural network modeling is discussed as an asset to research on cognitive development. 相似文献
138.
139.
Kevin Reuter 《Philosophia》2011,39(1):31-38
The literature suggests that in sensory imagination we focus on the imagined objects, not on the imaginative states themselves,
and that therefore imagination is not introspective. It is claimed that the introspection of imaginative states is an additional
cognitive ability. However, there seem to be counterexamples to this claim. In many cases in which we sensorily imagine a
certain object in front of us, we are aware that this object is not really where we imagine it to be. So it looks as if in
these cases of imagination, we are aware of the mere appearance of the imagined object, and hence introspection is a constitutive
part of imagination. In this article, I address this contradictory state of affairs and argue that we should classify at least
some forms of sensory imagination as introspective. For this purpose I use the appearance-reality distinction as a central
notion for introspection. I also defend the thesis of introspective imagination against the objection that young children
imagine without yet understanding the concept of experience. 相似文献
140.
Ockham’s razor is the principle that, all other things being equal, scientists ought to prefer simpler theories. In recent
years, philosophers have argued that simpler theories make better predictions, possess theoretical virtues like explanatory
power, and have other pragmatic virtues like computational tractability. However, such arguments fail to explain how and why
a preference for simplicity can help one find true theories in scientific inquiry, unless one already assumes that the truth is simple. One new solution to that problem is
the Ockham efficiency theorem (Kelly 2002, Minds Mach 14:485–505, 2004, Philos Sci 74:561–573, 2007a, b, Theor Comp Sci 383:270–289, c, d; Kelly and Glymour 2004), which states that scientists who heed Ockham’s razor retract their opinions less often and sooner than do their non-Ockham
competitors. The theorem neglects, however, to consider competitors following random (“mixed”) strategies and in many applications
random strategies are known to achieve better worst-case loss than deterministic strategies. In this paper, we describe two
ways to extend the result to a very general class of random, empirical strategies. The first extension concerns expected retractions,
retraction times, and errors and the second extension concerns retractions in chance, times of retractions in chance, and
chances of errors. 相似文献